首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   3456篇
  免费   189篇
  2023年   23篇
  2022年   16篇
  2021年   43篇
  2020年   83篇
  2019年   88篇
  2018年   108篇
  2017年   110篇
  2016年   118篇
  2015年   100篇
  2014年   98篇
  2013年   375篇
  2012年   170篇
  2011年   164篇
  2010年   106篇
  2009年   84篇
  2008年   152篇
  2007年   163篇
  2006年   135篇
  2005年   125篇
  2004年   122篇
  2003年   117篇
  2002年   116篇
  2001年   65篇
  2000年   49篇
  1999年   59篇
  1998年   42篇
  1997年   42篇
  1996年   37篇
  1995年   40篇
  1994年   40篇
  1993年   37篇
  1992年   37篇
  1991年   32篇
  1990年   36篇
  1989年   32篇
  1988年   28篇
  1987年   27篇
  1986年   31篇
  1985年   28篇
  1984年   30篇
  1983年   26篇
  1982年   35篇
  1981年   30篇
  1980年   22篇
  1979年   28篇
  1978年   22篇
  1977年   19篇
  1976年   32篇
  1975年   17篇
  1974年   23篇
排序方式: 共有3645条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
961.
The emergence of competing modules in bilingualism   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
How does the brain manage to store and process multiple languages without encountering massive interference and transfer? Unless we believe that bilinguals live in two totally unconnected cognitive worlds, we would expect far more transfer than actually occurs. However, imaging and lesion studies have not provided consistent evidence for the strict neuronal separation predicted by the theory of modularity. We suggest that emergentist theory offers a promising alternative. It emphasizes the competitive interplay between multiple languages during childhood and by focusing on the dual action of competition and entrenchment, avoids the need to invoke a critical period to account for age of acquisition effects in second-language learning. This view instantiates the motto formulated by Elizabeth Bates that 'modules are made, not born.'  相似文献   
962.
963.
Right hemisphere damaged patients with and without left visual neglect, and age-matched controls had objects of various sizes presented within left or right body hemispace. Subjects were asked to estimate the objects' sizes or to reach out and grasp them, in order to assess visual size processing in perceptual-experiential and action-based contexts respectively. No impairments of size processing were detected in the prehension performance of the neglect patients but a generalised slowing of movement was observed, associated with an extended deceleration phase. Additionally both patient groups reached maximum grip aperture relatively later in the movement than did controls. For the estimation task it was predicted that the left visual neglect group would systematically underestimate the sizes of objects presented within left hemispace but no such abnormalities were observed. Possible reasons for this unexpected null finding are discussed.  相似文献   
964.
Roberts MW 《心理评价》2001,13(1):46-58
Observing structured parent-child interaction in clinic analogs has been a tradition in child clinical assessment since the 1960s. The clinic analog is designed to re-create important conditions from natural contexts such that dysfunctional parent-child patterns can be observed and modified. To those basic goals, the modern clinician would add diagnosis and treatment evaluation. Three classes of parent-child clinic analogs designed to evaluate preadolescent children with externalizing disorders were reviewed: free play, parent-directed play, and parent-directed chores. Free-play analogs and parent-directed chore analogs were found to have merit, but remain psychometrically underdeveloped. Practitioner reliance on questionnaires and interviews appears likely to continue until observational analogs have attained sufficient psychometric qualities to facilitate routine clinical decisions.  相似文献   
965.
In this paper we review three prospective longitudinal studies from our laboratory examining the effects of prenatal stress on early neuro behavior, stress reactivity and learning performance in rhesus monkeys. Either a noise stressor or ACTH treatment was administered to pregnant monkeys during specific periods of pregnancy and offspring were examined repeatedly across development. In all three studies, the prenatally stressed monkeys showed reduced attention and impaired neuromotor functioning during the first month of life compared to controls from undisturbed pregnancies. When the monkeys were separated from their mothers or peers at 6-8 months of age, prenatally stressed monkeys exhibited more disturbance behavior and showed hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregulation. During adolescence, they exhibited impairments in learning, compared to controls.  相似文献   
966.
Two rat experiments shed light on how variation in behavior is regulated. Experiment 1 used the peak procedure. On most trials, the 1st bar press more than 40 s after signal onset ended the signal and produced food. Other trials lasted much longer and ended without food. On those trials, the variability of bar-press duration increased greatly after the 1st response more than 40 s after signal onset. In Experiment 2, which asked whether the increase was due to the omission of expected reward or the decrease in reward expectation, reward expectation had a strong effect on response duration, whereas omission of expected reward had little effect. In both experiments, response rate and response duration changed independently, suggesting that they reflect different parts of the underlying mechanism. In Experiment 1, response durations implied that timing of the signal was more accurate than the rate-vs.-time function might suggest. Experiment 2 suggested that lowering reward expectation increases variation in response form.  相似文献   
967.
Natural theology     
Brian Scarlett 《Sophia》2001,40(2):7-13
If the theological virtues are supernatural they must be said to be in some sense not natural. This suggests the possibility that they are not only not natural but positively unnatural, in that they postulate either an inhumanly high level of achievement or a divine takeover of human life. The solution proposed draws on Peter Forrest’s work inGod Without the Supernatural: A Defence of Scientific Theism, and suggests a naturalistic account of the virtues in question.  相似文献   
968.
969.
Several lines of research suggest there is considerable overlap between anxiety and depression and that it is difficult to distinguish between these two constructs. However, a few studies utilizing factor analytic procedures have provided evidence that anxiety and depression can be differentiated when measures of these constructs are considered at the item level. In addition, there is some evidence that differentiation can be accomplished in samples experiencing high levels of anxiety (i.e., a clinically anxious sample; B. J. Cox, R. P. Swinson, L. Kuch, & J. Reichman, 1993). In the present study, this research strategy was extended to a sample of patients with high levels of depressed mood (i.e., a mood disorders sample; N = 378). Their responses to widely used measures of depression (i.e., Beck Depression Inventory; A. T. Beck, C. H. Ward, M. Mendelson, J. Mock, & J. Erbaugh, 1961) and anxiety (i.e., Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory—State subscale; C. D. Spielberger, R. L. Gorsuch, & R. E. Lushene, 1970) were entered into a principal-components analysis with oblique rotation. A 4-factor solution was retained. This solution was comprised of factors representing anxiety, anxiety absent (a reverse scored factor), cognitive symptoms of depression, and somatic/vegetative symptoms of depression. These findings indicated that anxiety and depression, as emotional states, can be differentiated within a mood disorders sample, using existing popular self-report measures. The clinical and research implications of these findings are briefly discussed.  相似文献   
970.
The Depression–Anxiety–Stress Scales (DASS; P. F. Lovibond & S. H. Lovibond, 1995) have shown considerable promise in their ability to differentially assess depression and anxiety symptoms. Most of the work to date has relied on normal or predominantly anxious clinical samples. To extend this research to a predominantly depressed sample, a psychiatric sample from a mood disorders program (N = 439) was used to investigate the factor structure of the DASS. Confirmatory factor analyses indicated that a 3-factor model for the 21-item DASS was supported. A 3-factor model with crossloading items for the 42-item version showed no practical improvement. Further confirmatory analyses evaluated the ability of item subsets from the DASS to represent the constructs specified in the tripartite model (L. A. Clark & D. Watson, 1991). Strong support was obtained for the DASS to represent the construct of anhedonia and physiological hyperarousal, and their relative importance is consistent with predictions of the tripartite model in understanding the relationship between anxiety and depression.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号