全文获取类型
收费全文 | 19122篇 |
免费 | 397篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
19520篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 194篇 |
2019年 | 291篇 |
2018年 | 339篇 |
2017年 | 300篇 |
2016年 | 353篇 |
2015年 | 252篇 |
2014年 | 338篇 |
2013年 | 1568篇 |
2012年 | 570篇 |
2011年 | 604篇 |
2010年 | 361篇 |
2009年 | 373篇 |
2008年 | 526篇 |
2007年 | 569篇 |
2006年 | 518篇 |
2005年 | 443篇 |
2004年 | 465篇 |
2003年 | 423篇 |
2002年 | 413篇 |
2001年 | 487篇 |
2000年 | 545篇 |
1999年 | 375篇 |
1998年 | 236篇 |
1997年 | 187篇 |
1996年 | 197篇 |
1995年 | 193篇 |
1994年 | 178篇 |
1993年 | 172篇 |
1992年 | 303篇 |
1991年 | 307篇 |
1990年 | 322篇 |
1989年 | 301篇 |
1988年 | 292篇 |
1987年 | 253篇 |
1986年 | 268篇 |
1985年 | 280篇 |
1984年 | 210篇 |
1983年 | 229篇 |
1982年 | 189篇 |
1979年 | 270篇 |
1978年 | 235篇 |
1975年 | 224篇 |
1974年 | 260篇 |
1973年 | 272篇 |
1972年 | 229篇 |
1971年 | 212篇 |
1969年 | 196篇 |
1968年 | 252篇 |
1967年 | 243篇 |
1966年 | 212篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
841.
842.
Domino EF 《Brain and cognition》2003,53(1):66-74
After overnight abstinence, tobacco smokers smoked an average nicotine yield cigarette and nonsmokers sham smoked an unlit placebo cigarette. EEG alpha(1), delta, and theta frequency amplitudes decreased, whereas alpha(2) and beta frequency amplitude increased. Short, middle (EP) and long latency ERP were also studied in nonsmokers and smokers just after smoking, and after overnight abstinence from tobacco. Short latency potentials were unaffected by tobacco smoking or abstinence. Middle and long-latency potentials were reduced during abstinence and enhanced immediately after tobacco smoking. These findings indicate that compared to nonsmokers smokers have a higher arousal level after smoking than when partially abstinent. Evidence for both normalization from tobacco abstinence as well as stimulation was obtained. 相似文献
843.
Most computational and neural-style models of contour completion (ie illusory and occluded contours) are based on interpolation: the filling in of an edge between two visible edges. The results of three experiments suggest an alternative conception, that units are formed as a result of extrapolation from visible edges. In three experiments, subjects reported illusory contours between standard illusory-contour inducing elements and forms that do not, by themselves, induce illusory contours. We suggest that these forms are not a special case of inducing elements but that they represent a different class--receiving elements. Receiving elements are forms that can receive an illusory contour but cannot generate one, and they can alter contour formation. In experiment 1, receiving elements increased the judged clarity of illusory contours. In experiment 2, illusory edges were seen to connect to corners, line ends, and even the edges of circles. Boundary formation in motion displays also appears to be based on extrapolation. In experiment 3, subjects reported that small moving dots altered the formation of spatiotemporally defined boundaries. Implications for higher-order operator and network models of boundary formation are discussed. 相似文献
844.
Dissociations among attention, perception, and awareness during object-substitution masking 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
When a visual target object is surrounded by four dots that onset at the same time as the target but remain visible after the target terminates, the four dots dramatically impair target discrimination performance. This phenomenon is called object-substitution masking , reflecting the hypothesis that both the target and the four dots are identified, but the representation of the four dots replaces the representation of the target object before the target can be reported. The present study used the event-related potential technique to demonstrate that a target masked in this manner is identified by the visual system and triggers a shift of attention. However, by the time attention is shifted to the target, only the mask remains visible, leading to impaired behavioral detection performance. These findings support the object-substitution hypothesis and provide new evidence that perception, attention, and awareness can be dissociated. 相似文献
845.
To the extent that assessment improves the effectiveness of treatment, prevention, or other services, it can be said to be effective. If an assessment is as effective as alternatives for improving treatment and less costly, it can be said to be cost-effective. If that improvement in the effectiveness of the service is monetary or monetizable, the assessment can be judged beneficial. And, if the sum of monetary and monetizable benefits of assessment exceeds the sum of the costs of treatment, the assessment can be said to be cost-beneficial. An overview of cost-related issues is followed by practical strategies that researchers and administrators can use to measure incremental costs, incremental effectiveness, and incremental benefits of adding psychological assessments to other psychological interventions. 相似文献
846.
A theory of diversity in speeded cognition, the difference engine, is proposed, in which information processing is represented
as a series of generic computational steps. Some individuals tend to perform all of these computations relatively quickly
and other individuals tend to perform them all relatively slowly, reflecting the existence of a general cognitive speed factor,
but the time required for response selection and execution is assumed to be independent of cognitive speed. The difference
engine correctly predicts the positively accelerated form of the relation between diversity of performance, as measured by
the standard deviation for the group, and task difficulty, as indexed by the mean response time (RT) for the group. In addition,
the difference engine correctly predicts approximately linear relations between the RTs of any individual and average performance
for the group, with the regression lines for fast individuals having slopes less than 1.0 (and positive intercepts) and the
regression lines for slow individuals having slopes greater than 1.0 (and negative intercepts). Similar predictions are made
for comparisons of slow, average, and fast subgroups, regardless of whether those subgroups are formed on the basis of differences
in ability, age, or health status. These predictions are consistent with evidence from studies of healthy young and older
adults as well as from studies of depressed and age-matched control groups. 相似文献
847.
Three experiments explore the role of working memory in motor skill acquisition and performance. Traditional theories postulate that skill acquisition proceeds through stages of knowing, which are initially declarative but later procedural. The reported experiments challenge that view and support an independent, parallel processing model, which predicts that procedural and declarative knowledge can be acquired separately and that the former does not depend on the availability of working memory, whereas, the latter does. The behaviour of these two processes was manipulated by providing or withholding visual (and auditory) appraisal of outcome feedback. Withholding feedback was predicted to inhibit the use of working memory to appraise success and, thus, prevent the formation of declarative knowledge without affecting the accumulation of procedural knowledge. While the first experiment failed to support these predictions, the second and third experiments demonstrated that procedural and declarative knowledge can be acquired independently. It is suggested that the availability of working memory is crucial to motor performance only when the learner has come to rely on its use. 相似文献
848.
849.
850.