全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2748篇 |
免费 | 50篇 |
专业分类
2798篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 20篇 |
2022年 | 12篇 |
2021年 | 37篇 |
2020年 | 69篇 |
2019年 | 72篇 |
2018年 | 89篇 |
2017年 | 81篇 |
2016年 | 94篇 |
2015年 | 79篇 |
2014年 | 79篇 |
2013年 | 292篇 |
2012年 | 141篇 |
2011年 | 127篇 |
2010年 | 74篇 |
2009年 | 63篇 |
2008年 | 122篇 |
2007年 | 137篇 |
2006年 | 108篇 |
2005年 | 101篇 |
2004年 | 99篇 |
2003年 | 94篇 |
2002年 | 83篇 |
2001年 | 38篇 |
2000年 | 36篇 |
1999年 | 35篇 |
1998年 | 32篇 |
1997年 | 33篇 |
1996年 | 31篇 |
1995年 | 31篇 |
1994年 | 32篇 |
1993年 | 30篇 |
1992年 | 30篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 25篇 |
1989年 | 24篇 |
1988年 | 22篇 |
1987年 | 19篇 |
1986年 | 24篇 |
1985年 | 20篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1982年 | 32篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 17篇 |
1975年 | 12篇 |
1974年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有2798条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
251.
Low-anger individuals are less reactive, both emotionally and behaviourally, to a large variety of situational primes to anger and aggression. Why this is so, from an affective processing perspective, has been largely conjectural. Four studies (total N=270) sought to link individual differences in anger to tendencies exhibited in basic affective processing tasks. On the basis of motivational factors and considerations, it was hypothesised that negative evaluations would differentially activate a psychological alarm system at low levels of anger, resulting in a pause that should be evident in the speed of making subsequent evaluations. Just such a pattern was evident in all studies. By contrast, high-anger individuals did not pause following their negative evaluations. In relation to this affective processing tendency, at least, dramatically different effects were observed among low- versus high-anger individuals. Implications for the personality-processing literature, theories of trait anger, and fast-acting regulatory processes are discussed. 相似文献
252.
Covert shifts of attention precede and direct overt eye movements to stimuli that are task relevant or physically salient. A growing body of evidence suggests that the learned value of perceptual stimuli strongly influences their attentional priority. For example, previously rewarded but otherwise irrelevant and inconspicuous stimuli capture covert attention involuntarily. It is unknown, however, whether stimuli also draw eye movements involuntarily as a consequence of their reward history. Here, we show that previously rewarded but currently task-irrelevant stimuli capture both attention and the eyes. Value-driven oculomotor capture was observed during unconstrained viewing, when neither eye movements nor fixations were required, and was strongly related to individual differences in visual working memory capacity. The appearance of a reward-associated stimulus came to evoke pupil dilation over the course of training, which provides physiological evidence that the stimuli that elicit value-driven capture come to serve as reward-predictive cues. These findings reveal a close coupling of value-driven attentional capture and eye movements that has broad implications for theories of attention and reward learning. 相似文献
253.
Liu G Austen EL Booth KS Fisher BD Argue R Rempel MI Enns JT 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2005,31(2):235-247
This study tested whether multiple-object tracking-the ability to visually index objects on the basis of their spatiotemporal history-is scene based or image based. Initial experiments showed equivalent tracking accuracy for objects in 2-D and 3-D motion. Subsequent experiments manipulated the speeds of objects independent of the speed of the scene as a whole. Results showed that tracking accuracy was influenced by object speed but not by scene speed. This held true whether the scene underwent translation, zoom, rotation, or even combinations of all 3 motions. A final series of experiments interfered with observers' ability to see a coherent scene by moving objects at different speeds from one another and by distorting the perception of 3-D space. These reductions in scene coherence led to reduced tracking accuracy, confirming that tracking is accomplished using a scene-based, or allocentric, frame of reference. 相似文献
254.
Haig BD 《The American psychologist》2005,60(4):344-345
255.
Schrag B 《Science and engineering ethics》2005,11(3):347-366
Ethical tasks faced by researchers in science and engineering as they engage in research include recognition of moral problems in their practice, finding solutions to those moral problems, judging moral actions and engaging in preventive ethics. Given these issues, appropriate pedagogical objectives for research ethics education include (1) teaching researchers to recognize moral issues in their research, (2) teaching researchers to solve practical moral problems in their research from the perspective of the moral agent, (3) teaching researchers how to make moral judgments about actions, and (4) learning to engage in preventive ethics. If web-based research ethics education is intended to be adequate and sufficient for research ethics education, then it must meet those objectives. However there are reasons to be skeptical that it can. 相似文献
256.
Brian Huss 《Synthese》2009,168(2):249-271
In this paper I look at three challenges to the very possibility of an ethics of belief and then show how they can be met.
The first challenge, from Thomas Kelly, says that epistemic rationality is not (merely) a form of instrumental rationality.
If this claim is true, then it will be difficult to develop an ethics of belief that does not run afoul of naturalism. The
second challenge is the Non-Voluntarism Argument, which holds that because we cannot believe at will and because ought implies
can, there can be no ethics of belief. The third challenge comes from Richard Feldman, who claims that there is no such thing
as ought all-things-considered. He says, for example, that moral oughts can be weighed against other moral oughts and that
epistemic oughts can be compared to each other, but that there is no way to weigh moral oughts against epistemic oughts. If
this is true, then norms about what one ought to believe are not nearly as important as one might have hoped or as philosophers
have traditionally thought. In answering these three challenges, I try to show how and why the project of developing epistemic
norms might be a promising avenue of research, despite claims to the contrary. 相似文献
257.
258.
This experiment investigated potential gender biases in the emergence of leadership in groups. Teams played a public-goods game under conditions of intra- or intergroup competition. We predicted and found a strong preference for female leaders during intragroup competition and male leaders during intergroup competition. Furthermore, during intragroup competition, a female leader was more instrumental than a male leader in raising group investments, but this pattern was reversed during intergroup competition. These findings suggest that particular group threats elicit specific gender-biased leader prototypes. We speculate about the evolutionary and cultural origins of these sex differences in the emergence of leadership. 相似文献
259.
A critical challenge for visual perception is to represent objects as the same persisting individuals over time and motion. Across several areas of cognitive science, researchers have identified cohesion as among the most important theoretical principles of object persistence: An object must maintain a single bounded contour over time. Drawing inspiration from recent work in adult visual cognition, the present study tested the power of cohesion as a constraint as it operates early in development. In particular, we tested whether the most minimal cohesion violation - a single object splitting into two - would destroy infants' ability to represent a quantity of objects over occlusion. In a forced-choice crawling paradigm, 10- and 12-month-old infants witnessed crackers being sequentially placed into containers, and typically crawled toward the container with the greater cracker quantity. When one of the crackers was visibly split in half, however, infants failed to represent the relative quantities, despite controls for the overall quantities and the motions involved. This result helps to characterize the fidelity and specificity of cohesion as a fundamental principle of object persistence, suggesting that even the simplest possible cohesion violation can dramatically impair infants' object representations and influence their overt behavior. 相似文献
260.
Brian W. Junker 《Psychometrika》1991,56(2):255-278
A definition ofessential independence is proposed for sequences of polytomous items. For items satisfying the reasonable assumption that the expected amount of credit awarded increases with examinee ability, we develop a theory ofessential unidimensionality which closely parallels that of Stout. Essentially unidimensional item sequences can be shown to have a unique (up to change-of-scale) dominant underlying trait, which can be consistently estimated by a monotone transformation of the sum of the item scores. In more general polytomous-response latent trait models (with or without ordered responses), anM-estimator based upon maximum likelihood may be shown to be consistent for under essentially unidimensional violations of local independence and a variety of monotonicity/identifiability conditions. A rigorous proof of this fact is given, and the standard error of the estimator is explored. These results suggest that ability estimation methods that rely on the summation form of the log likelihood under local independence should generally be robust under essential independence, but standard errors may vary greatly from what is usually expected, depending on the degree of departure from local independence. An index of departure from local independence is also proposed.This work was supported in part by Office of Naval Research Grant N00014-87-K-0277 and National Science Foundation Grant NSF-DMS-88-02556. The author is grateful to William F. Stout for many helpful comments, and to an anonymous reviewer for raising the questions addressed in section 2. A preliminary version of section 6 appeared in the author's Ph.D. thesis. 相似文献