全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3749篇 |
免费 | 348篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
4098篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 54篇 |
2022年 | 12篇 |
2021年 | 36篇 |
2020年 | 154篇 |
2019年 | 84篇 |
2018年 | 93篇 |
2017年 | 147篇 |
2016年 | 168篇 |
2015年 | 145篇 |
2014年 | 142篇 |
2013年 | 578篇 |
2012年 | 177篇 |
2011年 | 139篇 |
2010年 | 114篇 |
2009年 | 126篇 |
2008年 | 144篇 |
2007年 | 156篇 |
2006年 | 137篇 |
2005年 | 103篇 |
2004年 | 101篇 |
2003年 | 100篇 |
2002年 | 87篇 |
2001年 | 42篇 |
2000年 | 42篇 |
1999年 | 41篇 |
1998年 | 43篇 |
1997年 | 39篇 |
1996年 | 69篇 |
1995年 | 59篇 |
1994年 | 53篇 |
1993年 | 53篇 |
1992年 | 60篇 |
1991年 | 54篇 |
1990年 | 57篇 |
1989年 | 41篇 |
1988年 | 38篇 |
1987年 | 30篇 |
1986年 | 35篇 |
1985年 | 22篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 39篇 |
1981年 | 28篇 |
1980年 | 25篇 |
1979年 | 34篇 |
1978年 | 36篇 |
1977年 | 32篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 13篇 |
1974年 | 16篇 |
排序方式: 共有4098条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
Little attention has been given to teaching adaptive community skills to retarded persons. In this study, five retarded male students were taught basic pedestrian skills in a classroom. Training was conducted on a model built to simulate city traffic conditions. Each subject was taught five specific skills involved in street crossing in sequence, viz. intersection recognition, pedestrian-light skills, traffic-light skills, and skills for two different stop-sign conditions. Before, during, and after training, subjects were tested on generalization probes on the model and under actual city traffic conditions. Results of a multiple-baseline design across both subjects and behaviors indicated that after receiving classroom training on the skills, each subject exhibited appropriate pedestrian skills under city traffic conditions. In addition, training in some skills appeared to facilitate performance in skills not yet trained. 相似文献
102.
Anthony J. Adams Brian Brown Merton C. Flom 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1976,19(3):219-225
Relatively low doses of alcohol produced large, significant, dose-related increases in the time required to recover foveal contrast sensitivity following bright light exposure. Nine subjects participated in a double-blind experiment involving three dose levels of alcohol (including placebo). The luminance parameters of the test were comparable to those encountered in practical situations such as driving. The alcohol-induced delay in glare recovery is probably retinal and lasts for several hours after drinking. 相似文献
103.
104.
105.
Mary M. Mitchell PhD Michael Shayne Gallaway PhD MA Amy M. Millikan MD MPH Michael Bell MD MPH 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2012,42(5):486-494
Suicide is one of the leading causes of death among U.S. Army soldiers. Suicide‐related ideation, which is associated with suicide attempts and suicide, can cause considerable distress. In a sample of 1,663 recently redeployed soldiers, we used factor analysis and structural equation modeling to test the associations between combat exposure, unit cohesion, and their interaction in predicting suicide‐related ideation. We found that combat exposure was a significant risk factor for suicide‐related ideation, while unit cohesion was a significant protective factor. The significant interaction between the two factors indicated that soldiers who experienced greater combat exposure but also had higher levels of unit cohesion had relatively lower levels of suicide‐related ideation. In addition, those who had higher levels of combat exposure and lower unit cohesion were most at risk for suicide‐related ideation. Our findings indicate the importance of unit cohesion in protecting soldiers from suicide‐related ideation and suggest a higher risk group of soldiers who should be targeted for interventions. 相似文献
106.
Kenneth R. Conner PsyD MPH Michael D. McCarthy PhD Alina Bajorska MA Eric D. Caine MD Xin M. Tu PhD Kerry L. Knox PhD 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2012,42(6):699-708
There are meager prospective data from nonclinical samples on the link between anxiety disorders and suicide or the extent to which the association varies over time. We examined these issues in a cohort of 309,861 U.S. Air Force service members, with 227 suicides over follow‐up. Mental disorder diagnoses including anxiety, mood, and substance‐use disorders (SUD) were based on treatment encounters. Risk for suicide associated with anxiety disorders were lower compared with mood disorders and similar to SUD. Moreover, the associations between mood and anxiety disorders with suicide were greatest within a year of treatment presentation. 相似文献
107.
Skea BR 《The Journal of analytical psychology》2006,51(4):527-552
Abstract: Since the 1982 publication of Aldo Carotenuto's book, A Secret Symmetry: Sabina Spielrein Between Jung and Freud, there has been renewed interest in the life and work of Sabina Spielrein. She was Jung's first psychoanalytic case at the Burghölzli Hospital in 1904, and was referred to several times in The Freud/Jung Letters. Spielrein recovered, enrolled in medical school, and went on to become a Freudian analyst. Her most famous paper, published in 1912, ‘Destruction as a cause of coming into being’, was referred to by Freud in 1920 in relation to his Death Instinct theory. In the few Freudian publications on this controversial theory since 1920, Spielrein's contribution is consistently omitted. Jung also neglected to refer to her ‘Destruction’ paper in his early 1912 version of ‘Symbols of transformation’, even though he had edited her paper and had promised to acknowledge her contribution. He did refer extensively to Spielrein's first paper, her medical thesis, ‘On the psychological content of a case of schizophrenia’, published in 1911, as yet unpublished in English. In her paper Spielrein sought to understand the psychotic delusions of Frau M, a patient at the Burghölzli, much in the style of Jung's ‘Psychology of dementia praecox’ (1907). The purpose of this paper is to explore to what extent Spielrein's Frau M paper, and its companion ‘Destruction’ paper, make an original contribution to both Jung and Freud's emerging theories on the possible creative versus destructive outcomes of neurotic or psychotic introversion, culminating in Jung's concept of the ‘collective unconscious’ (1916) and Freud's concept of a ‘Death instinct’ (1920). 相似文献
108.
This paper describes a 7.5 year retrospective study of all patients discharged from inpatient forensic services to forensic community team (FCT) follow-up from the Auckland Regional Forensic Psychiatry Service. Patients' files were studied for clinical, criminal, and risk data, type of service delivered, and final level of function achieved in the community. Rearrest, re-hospitalization, and reimprisonment data were obtained from clinical, court, and prison records.105 patients were included. The most common diagnosis was a psychotic disorder, and index offending behaviour was typically violent. The median period of inpatient stay was 36 months and mean subsequent FCT follow-up 21.7 months (SD 17.8). The majority of patients were from Maori and Pacific Island ethnic groups. At the end of the study, half were in independent living, half were in some form of employment, and 19% were readmitted to a forensic hospital.One patient was rearrested but not reimprisoned whilst under forensic community team care. However, 9 of the 48 who were discharged to general mental health services were rearrested and 5 reimprisoned. Only two offences were as serious as the original index offence.Broad based assertive, mandated, and committed forensic rehabilitation can achieve high quality outcomes. These levels of function may not be sustained under less assertive care. 相似文献
109.
HOW TO PREDICT FUTURE DURATION FROM PRESENT AGE 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The physicist J. Richard Gott has given an argument which, if good, allows one to make accurate predictions for the future longevity of a process, based solely on its present age. We show that there are problems with some of the details of Gott's argument, but we defend the core thesis: in many circumstances, the greater the present age of a process, the more likely a longer future duration. 相似文献
110.
Spatial updating relies on an egocentric representation of space: Effects of the number of objects 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Wang RF Crowell JA Simons DJ Irwin DE Kramer AF Ambinder MS Thomas LE Gosney JL Levinthal BR Hsieh BB 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2006,13(2):281-286
Models of spatial updating attempt to explain how representations of spatial relationships between the actor and objects in
the environment change as the actor moves. In allocentric models, object locations are encoded in an external reference frame,
and only the actor’s position and orientation in that reference frame need to be updated. Thus, spatial updating should be
independent of the number of objects in the environment (set size). In egocentric updating models, object locations are encoded
relative to the actor, so the location of each object relative to the actor must be updated as the actor moves. Thus, spatial
updating efficiency should depend on set size. We examined which model better accounts for human spatial updating by having
people reconstruct the locations of varying numbers of virtual objects either from the original study position or from a changed
viewing position. In consistency with the egocentric updating model, object localization following a viewpoint change was
affected by the number of objects in the environment. 相似文献