全文获取类型
收费全文 | 54787篇 |
免费 | 939篇 |
国内免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
55730篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 749篇 |
2019年 | 947篇 |
2018年 | 1064篇 |
2017年 | 1077篇 |
2016年 | 1321篇 |
2015年 | 954篇 |
2014年 | 944篇 |
2013年 | 4564篇 |
2012年 | 1671篇 |
2011年 | 1835篇 |
2010年 | 1071篇 |
2009年 | 1081篇 |
2008年 | 1655篇 |
2007年 | 1623篇 |
2006年 | 1427篇 |
2005年 | 1304篇 |
2004年 | 1271篇 |
2003年 | 1157篇 |
2002年 | 1188篇 |
2001年 | 1763篇 |
2000年 | 1656篇 |
1999年 | 1265篇 |
1998年 | 674篇 |
1997年 | 581篇 |
1996年 | 523篇 |
1995年 | 522篇 |
1994年 | 501篇 |
1993年 | 520篇 |
1992年 | 1014篇 |
1991年 | 956篇 |
1990年 | 945篇 |
1989年 | 853篇 |
1988年 | 819篇 |
1987年 | 807篇 |
1986年 | 806篇 |
1985年 | 858篇 |
1984年 | 669篇 |
1983年 | 581篇 |
1982年 | 472篇 |
1979年 | 656篇 |
1978年 | 492篇 |
1976年 | 449篇 |
1975年 | 592篇 |
1974年 | 624篇 |
1973年 | 595篇 |
1972年 | 540篇 |
1971年 | 477篇 |
1968年 | 541篇 |
1967年 | 545篇 |
1966年 | 477篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
851.
This experiment investigated Allport's (1977) claim that normal subjects can be induced to make semantic errors of the type characterised by “deep dyslexia”. The procedure followed that used by Allport except that a subject based control for the chance level of semantic errors was included. The results replicated the original finding by producing a semantic error rate of 12·7%. It was shown, however, that this rate was not significantly higher than the calculated chance rate of 11·5%. It was concluded, therefore, that the semantic error rates reported by Allport can be accounted for in terms of a guessing artefact and thus they bear no relevance to the problem of deep dyslexia. 相似文献
852.
S. A. Corson E. O’L. Corson R. E. Becker B. E. Ginsburg A. Trattner R. L. Connor L. A. Lucas J. Panksepp J. P. Scott 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1980,15(1):5-11
A model of childhood hyperkinesis, using the Telomian-beagle hybrid dog, is described, demonstrating the interactions of genetics and separation in the development of this syndrome and in the favorable responses of some of these dogs to amphetamine. This work represents the collaboration of three laboratories, utilizing chiefly methods of inhibitory training. Inducing separation distress enhanced the resistance to inhibitory training and the favorable responses to amphetamine. 相似文献
853.
Robert Barcus Andrew I. Schwebel Samuel A. Corson 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1980,15(4):183-187
This report describes the use of an animal model for testing the relationship between the effects of ingesting food additives and hyperactivity. Telomian × beagle hybrid dogs were chosen as subjects because this breed has been observed to exhibit behavior seen as analogous to the hyperactivity of children. Two studies were conducted by adding or deleting butylated hydroxyanisole and Food, Drug, and Cosmetic red dye number 40 to the diet of each dog. A test of sitting was performed to measure the level of activity. While a relationship was not observed between the ingestion of the additive and the level of activity, several issues pertaining to the use of the animal model became apparent. These issues and the use of animals for future research in this area are discussed. 相似文献
854.
A simple dc shock-generating device is described. The principle of operation relies upon the mutual inductance effect between the primary and secondary coils of a transformer. 相似文献
855.
856.
Given sequences of digits with temporally equidistant acoustic onsets, listeners do not perceive them as isochronous (Morton, Marcus, & Frankish, 1976). In order for the sequences to be perceptually isochronous, systematic departures from acoustic isochrony must be introduced. These acoustic departures are precisely those that talkers generate when asked to produce an isochronous sequence (Fowler, 1979), suggesting that listeners judge isochrony based on acoustic information about articulatory timing. The present experiment was an attempt to test directly whether perceptually isochronous sequences have isochronous articulatory correlates. Electromyographic potentials were recorded from the orbicularis oris muscle when speakers produced sequences of monosyllables “as if speaking in time to a metronome.” Sequences were devised so that lip-muscle activity was related to the syllable-initial consonant, the stressed vowel, or the stressed vowel and final consonant. Results indicate that isochronous muscular activity accompanies both isochronous and anisochronous acoustic signals produced under instructions to generate isochronous sequences. These results support an interpretation of the perceptual phenomenon reported by Morton et al. to the effect that listeners judge isochrony of the talker’s articulations as they are reflected in the acoustic signal. 相似文献
857.
J. A. Scott Kelso Kenneth G. Holt Adrian E. Flatt 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1980,28(1):45-52
The theoretical role of proprioception in the perception and control of human movement is elusive because of the obvious inability to manipulate experimentally the various receptive systems. Individuals who have had the metacarpophalangeal joint and joint capsule removed and replaced with silastic inserts afford a unique opportunity to evaluate a principal source of proprioception, namely, slowly adapting joint afferents. In a set of experiments, we show that such individuals show no deficits in finger localization following joint replacement. We take this and other complementary findings as a preliminary basis for proposing a dynamic rather than kinematic account of movement production. In addition, we provide a reconceptualization of the function of proprioceptive information in the CNS. Our arguments focus on proprioceptive inputs as tuning or modulating interneuronal pools rather than providing dimension-pecific information to the brain, as is commonly assumed. 相似文献
858.
859.
Context dependent changes in the reinforcing strength of schedule-induced drinking. 总被引:6,自引:6,他引:0
下载免费PDF全文

Previous experiments show that the opportunity to engage in schedule-induced responding is reinforcing. In this experiment, the reinforcing strength of schedule-induced drinking was measured. Four rats were trained on a concurrent-chain schedule. The two terminal links provided food pellets on identical fixed-time schedules. In addition, one terminal link also provided the opportunity to press a button that operated a water dipper. In this link, the rats showed polydipsic drinking. Button-pressing rate for polydipsic drinking was a bitonic function of pellet rate, and it was possible to describe the relationship with a slightly modified version of the matching equation for primary reinforcement. This equation also closely fit the data from other studies. Initial-link response rates, however, did not appear to be influenced by the availability of water in the terminal links. Control conditions suggested that the reinforcing strength of polydipsia was strongly bound to the context provided by periodic food reinforcement. 相似文献
860.
The experimental analysis of behavior has grown enormously in Latin America within the last decade. This paper offers an updating revision of suggested Spanish translations of operant expressions. 相似文献