首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   21209篇
  免费   51篇
  2022年   8篇
  2021年   7篇
  2020年   11篇
  2019年   24篇
  2018年   3494篇
  2017年   2835篇
  2016年   2268篇
  2015年   206篇
  2014年   91篇
  2013年   146篇
  2012年   593篇
  2011年   2404篇
  2010年   2529篇
  2009年   1492篇
  2008年   1726篇
  2007年   2192篇
  2006年   49篇
  2005年   226篇
  2004年   191篇
  2003年   138篇
  2002年   76篇
  2001年   55篇
  2000年   77篇
  1999年   30篇
  1998年   28篇
  1997年   28篇
  1996年   14篇
  1995年   10篇
  1994年   14篇
  1992年   13篇
  1991年   12篇
  1990年   26篇
  1989年   19篇
  1988年   12篇
  1986年   10篇
  1985年   15篇
  1984年   14篇
  1983年   12篇
  1982年   13篇
  1981年   17篇
  1980年   9篇
  1979年   10篇
  1977年   13篇
  1975年   11篇
  1974年   6篇
  1973年   9篇
  1969年   6篇
  1968年   13篇
  1967年   9篇
  1966年   7篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Simple motor reaction times (right-handed) to tape-recorded consonant-vowel syllables were obtained from 12 subjects under two conditions of monotic stimulation: “expected” presentation (subject informed as to ear of presentation) and “random” presentation (ear of presentation varied randomly). Significantly faster left ear reaction times were obtained in the “expected” condition. The rank order of subjects' standard dichotic listening task scores correlated negatively with reaction time ear differences in the “expected” presentation condition. Results are discussed in terms of existing theories of response lateralization in simple reaction time tasks, and an expanded hypothesis is offered. Specifically, it is suggested that two or more distinct but potentially overlapping mechanisms may be responsible for observed asymmetries in monotic auditory perception. Both an attentional or lateralized motor response bias and an inherent lateralization of function may operate side by side, differentially activated by task demands, mode of stimulus presentation, and nature of stimuli.  相似文献   
62.
Three linguists reliably judged the accentedness of 32 Mexican-Americans and four Anglo male readers using a 7-point Preliminary Accentedness Scale. Next, the judges provided reliable assessments of the relative frequency of 18 types of accented pronunciation for nine of the speakers. An Accentedness Index was developed by averaging the relative frequencies over the 18 pronunciations. Accentedness measures produced by the linguists for the nine test speakers were compared with ratings of status, solidarity, and accent given by naive raters in another experiment (Brennan and Brennan, 1978). The Accentedness Index was a significant predictor of the Preliminary Accentedness Scale, of accentedness assessments by the naive listeners, and of status judgments. Pronunciation types were significant predictors both of accent and of status. Results are discussed in terms of validation of the concept of accentedness. Future use of accentedness measures to investigate spontaneous speech and attitudes of listeners toward their own speech is suggested.The present investigation is based upon data collected by the first author for her doctoral dissertation at the university of Notre Dame, and was partially supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health under Grant NICHD-06921. A preliminary version of this paper was presented at the Linguistic Attitudes Session of the Ninth World Congress of Sociology, Uppsala, Sweden, in August 1978.  相似文献   
63.
Self-report measures of grief, depression, and general psychopathology were studied in widows and widowers over a 2.5-year period following death of their partner. A comparison sample of men and women was also followed for the same period. Differences in severity of depression and psychopathology previously reported at 2 months postloss (Gallagher, Breckenridge, Thompson, & Peterson, 1983) diminished to nonsignificant levels at 12 and 30 months. However, significant differences between bereaved and comparison subjects on measures of grief were still apparent 30 months after spousal loss. A main effect of gender for depression and psychopathology (but not for grief) was found at 2 and 12 months: Women reported more distress than men regardless of bereavement status. Results indicate that the experience of grief persists for at least 30 months in both older men and women who have lost their spouse.  相似文献   
64.
Reviews     
M asterson , J. F. The Narcissistic and Borderline Disorders
T ustin , F. (Ed.). Austistic States in Children
M iller , D avid L. The New Polytheism: Rebirth of the Gods and Goddesses
S haffer , H.; B urglass , M. E. (Eds). Classic Contributions in Addictions
S chneiderman , L. The Psychology of Myth, Folklore, and Religion
O lney , J ames . The Rhizome and the Flower. The Perennial Philosophy—Yeats and Jung
B ickman , M artin . The Unsounded Centre: Jungian Studies in American Romanticism  相似文献   
65.
66.
Video records of five different sessions of a single therapy group were scored for the occurrence of laughing, smiling, and talking. Successive five-minute intervals were also scored for therapeutic level through ratings derived from the Hill Interaction Matrix. Humorous remarks were categorized according to humor target: self, other in group, and generalized other. Results indicated that the vast majority of humorous remarks were directed at some specific target, and that over 50% of these remarks were negative in tone. Results also revealed that remarks targeted at others in the group tended to decrease therapeutic effectiveness whereas remarks targeted at individuals or institutions not presently in the group were found to increase therapeutic effectiveness. Selftargeted remarks were found to produce inconsistent effects. These findings are discussed in terms of their significance for a more general analysis of group humor as well as in terms of their more specific implications for therapeutic interventions.  相似文献   
67.
68.
69.
70.
Most personality tests are based on concepts assessing typical performance, and tests of this sort have not been generally successful in predicting criterion behaviors with useful levels of efficiency. Ability tests, which call for maximal performance, have been much more successful as predictors of criterion outcomes. Following a model derived from ability tests, predictors requiring separate typical and maximal self-reports of emotional expressivity were compared to measures of typical and maximal emotional expression in the laboratory. For angry expression, self-reports of maximal expressivity tended to outpredict self-reports of typical expressivity for both typical and maximal laboratory measures of angry expression. Although similar trends were observed for elation, the advantage of maximal self-report measures over typical self-report measures was negligible. Results were discussed in terms of both experimental and clinical implications.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号