首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   308篇
  免费   14篇
  2023年   3篇
  2022年   2篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   13篇
  2017年   14篇
  2016年   11篇
  2015年   8篇
  2014年   10篇
  2013年   46篇
  2012年   18篇
  2011年   13篇
  2010年   10篇
  2009年   13篇
  2008年   18篇
  2007年   10篇
  2006年   15篇
  2005年   11篇
  2004年   7篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   5篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   3篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   3篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   1篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   2篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   2篇
  1977年   5篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   4篇
  1973年   1篇
  1971年   5篇
  1970年   1篇
  1968年   3篇
  1967年   3篇
  1966年   2篇
  1965年   2篇
排序方式: 共有322条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
Coloring only a single letter in the Stroop task can result in a reduction or elimination of Stroop interference. The present experiments were designed to test whether this modulation of Stroop interference occurs at all letter positions. Specifically, we investigated whether Stroop interference was reduced when the colored letter occupied the optimal viewing position (OVP). The experiments show that Stroop interference is not reduced at the OVP (Experiment 1) and that Stroop interference at the OVP is significantly greater than at other letter positions (Experiments 1 and 2). This finding has important theoretical and methodological consequences for studies of automatic processing in visual word recognition.  相似文献   
53.
Prior research suggests children’s involvement in organized activities predicts positive outcomes, although benefits for youth with psychosocial deficits remain unclear. This study examines if activity participation predicts response to a group therapy program for children with psychosocial deficits and if improvements depend on children’s functioning within the organized activity context. Participants were 178 children (M age?=?9.6 years) enrolled in a group therapy program. Greater activity participation significantly predicted improvements in family functioning and social skills, suggesting the value of participation in organized activities for youth with psychosocial deficits.  相似文献   
54.
55.
The issue addressed in this study is whether there are differential effects of number of letters on word and nonword naming latency. Experiment 1 examined the effect of number of letters on latency for naming high-frequency words, low-frequency words, and nonwords. Number of letters affected latency for low-frequency words and nonwords but did not affect latency for high-frequency words. Number of letters was also negatively correlated with number of orthographic neighbours, number of friends, and average grapheme frequency. Number of letters continued to affect nonword naming latency, but not low-frequency word naming latency, after the effects of orthographic neighbourhood size, number of friends, and average grapheme frequency had been accounted for. Experiment 2 found that number of letters had no effect on the latency of delayed naming of the same words and nonwords. It is concluded that the effect of number of letters on nonword naming reflects a sequential, non-lexical reading mechanism.  相似文献   
56.
57.
58.
Patients with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) without concurrent depression (n = 11) and normal controls (n = 17) were tested twice, about 2 months apart, on a modified Stroop colour-naming task, which presented anxiety-related, depression-related and neutral words in masked and unmasked exposure conditions. GAD patients received cognitive behaviour therapy in the test-retest interval, and were also retested at follow-up, about 20 months after initial testing. GAD patients showed interference in colour-naming negative words across both masked and unmasked conditions before treatment, but not post-treatment, compared with controls. Reduced interference effects of masked threat words over time correlated with reduced ratings of anxious thoughts at post-treatment, and at follow-up, in GAD patients. Thus, the preconscious bias for threat information in GAD appears to vary over time in association with changes in anxious thoughts and worries.  相似文献   
59.
60.
Five squirrel monkeys showed a significant Symbolic Distance Effect (SDE) when tested on procedures designed to incorporate reaction time (RT) measures within the five-term series (“transitive inference”) task. Taken together with the high levels of transitive choice obtained by these subjects, this result shows that monkeys have met choice and reaction time criteria perceived by many as indexical of “linear representation”. Explicit evaluation of the representational implications of the SDE was subsequently undertaken in three-choice transfer tests, which initially revealed consistent deviations from the ranks predicted on a linear model. Extensive testing with the triadic method, however, elicited spontaneous improvement in overall choice ranking according to a linear principle. Further decomposition of the RT data showed that the irregularities in the initial transfer behaviour are by no means paradoxical. The ordinal separation data structure, from which the SDE is derived, masks two separate populations of scores--one fast and relatively undifferentiated, the other slow and based on judgements with poorly encoded items. And it is these factors, not inter-item distance, which, we argue, are the main determiners of the SDE. Whilst this phenomenon is thus only weakly indexical of principled linear representation, we contend that new (triad-based) ranking evidence suggests that monkeys may be capable of seriation as well as transitivity.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号