全文获取类型
收费全文 | 395篇 |
免费 | 22篇 |
专业分类
417篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 16篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 17篇 |
2013年 | 61篇 |
2012年 | 26篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 15篇 |
2008年 | 21篇 |
2007年 | 9篇 |
2006年 | 21篇 |
2005年 | 14篇 |
2004年 | 16篇 |
2003年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 6篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有417条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
82.
The role of the media in body image concerns among women: a meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Research suggests that exposure to mass media depicting the thin-ideal body may be linked to body image disturbance in women. This meta-analysis examined experimental and correlational studies testing the links between media exposure to women's body dissatisfaction, internalization of the thin ideal, and eating behaviors and beliefs with a sample of 77 studies that yielded 141 effect sizes. The mean effect sizes were small to moderate (ds = -.28, -.39, and -.30, respectively). Effects for some outcome variables were moderated by publication year and study design. The findings support the notion that exposure to media images depicting the thin-ideal body is related to body image concerns for women. 相似文献
83.
84.
Approximately 40-60% of BXSB mice have neocortical ectopias, a developmental anomaly characterized by migration of neurons into the neuron-sparse layer I of cortex. Previous studies have shown that ectopic BXSB mice have superior reference, but inferior working, memory on spatial tasks. Female BXSB mice were housed either in an enriched environment or in standard cages at weaning. Subsequently, these animals were tested on four of the Hebb-Williams mazes in a water-based version of this maze. Theoretically, two of the maze configurations placed greater emphasis on reference memory to find the goal, whereas the other two favored working memory. Ectopics reared in standard housing conditions were better than nonectopics on mazes that favored the use of reference memory, but poorer on mazes that favored working memory. In contrast, subjects raised in the enriched environment showed no ectopia differences. A comparison of enriched and standard housing conditions found that the enriched animals had better reference memory but poorer working memory. The latter effect may be because the enriched environment, although more stimulating, did not change in time or space; and other researchers have shown that daily replacement of stimuli in complex environments is correlated with better working memory. 相似文献
85.
Previous studies have suggested that the effects of extinction are response-specific. The present study investigated whether
an extinction treatment that eliminated goal tracking elicited by an appetitive conditioned stimulus (CS) would also eliminate
the conditioned reinforcing effects of that CS. Rats were first trained on a goal-tracking procedure in which an auditory
CS was paired with a food unconditioned stimulus. Animals learned to approach the location where the food was delivered. In
a subsequent phase, rats in one group received extinction training that eliminated the goal-tracking elicited by the CS. Rats
in the other group did not experience extinction of the food-paired CS. Then, both groups received a test for conditioned
reinforcement in which leverpresses resulted in the brief presentation of the stimulus previously paired with food. This stimulus
did not act as a conditioned reinforcer in the group that had been subjected to extinction training, but did serve as a conditioned
reinforcer in the group that did not experience extinction. These results indicate that the effects of extinction generalize
from the approach-eliciting to the conditioned reinforcing effects of an appetitive CS. 相似文献
86.
Intraindividual personality variability is a construct that reflects the extent to which a person's self-reported personality changes over time or across social roles. Past studies have linked variability with important outcomes such as adjustment and well-being. However, existing variability measures conflate mean-level variance with true change over time, and thus these past findings are questionable. Three studies were conducted to examine the psychometric properties of existing variability indexes and to develop a new index that does not suffer from the problem of conflated variance. This new index is reliable and valid and can predict actual changes in self-reports over time. However, once mean-level variance is removed, intraindividual variability is no longer related to well-being. 相似文献
87.
Jolanda Jetten Brendan J. McAuliffe Matthew J. Hornsey Michael A. Hogg 《European journal of social psychology》2006,36(6):825-843
The power of individualist and collectivist group norms to influence intergroup and inter‐individual differentiation was examined in three studies. Study 1 revealed that intergroup differentiation was lower when group norms prescribed individualism than when they prescribed collectivism. However, inter‐individual differentiation was higher when group norms endorsed individualism than when they promoted collectivism. In Studies 2 and 3 we found evidence for the moderating effect of group salience on the relationship between norms and differentiation. Specifically, the effect that individualist group norms reduced intergroup differentiation but enhanced inter‐individual differentiation was more pronounced when group salience was high rather than low. This finding demonstrates that conformity to a group norm prescribing individualism influences the manner in which positive differentiation is expressed. The discussion focuses on the caveats of introducing individualist group norms when attempting to reduce intergroup differentiation. Copyright © 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
88.
This paper discusses the reasons for the persistence of the disease model of alcoholism in the face of increasing evidence contradicting its basic assumptions. Data from in‐depth interviews with former problem drinkers are used to illustrate the ways in which the disease model: 1) shapes the empirical reality from which data for its support are derived, 2) implies methodological strategies which limit the possibility of its refutation and 3) blinds its supporters to the meaning of anomalous data. Implications for the immediate future of the disease model are discussed. 相似文献
89.
Brendan A. Rich Melissa A. Brotman Daniel P. Dickstein Derek G. V. Mitchell R. James R. Blair Ellen Leibenluft 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2010,38(5):695-706
Studying attention in the context of emotional stimuli may aid in differentiating pediatric bipolar disorder (BD) from severe
mood dysregulation (SMD). SMD is characterized by chronic irritability, arousal, and hyper-reactivity; SMD youth frequently
receive a BD diagnosis although they do not meet DSM-IV criteria for BD because they lack manic episodes. We compared 57 BD
(14.4 ± 2.9 years old, 56% male), 41 SMD (12.6 ± 2.6 years old, 66% male), and 33 control subjects (13.7 ± 2.5 years old,
52% male) using the Emotional Interrupt task, which examines how attention is impacted by positive, negative, or neutral distracters.
We compared reaction time (RT) and accuracy and calculated attention interference scores by subtracting performance on neutral
trials from emotional trials. Between-group analyses indicated that SMD subjects had significantly reduced attention interference
from emotional distracters relative to BD and control subjects. Thus, attention in SMD youth was not modulated by emotional
stimuli. This blunted response in SMD youth may contribute to their affective and behavioral dysregulation. 相似文献
90.
The permutation test follows directly from the procedure in a comparative experiment, does not depend on a known distribution
for error, and is sometimes more sensitive to real effects than are the corresponding parametric tests. Despite its advantages,
the permutation test is seldom (if ever) applied to factorial designs because of the computational load that they impose.
We propose two methods to limit the computation load. We show, first, that orthogonal contrasts limit the computational load
and, second, that when combined with Gill’s (2007) algorithm, the factorial permutation test is both practical and efficient.
For within-subjects designs, the factorial permutation test is equivalent to an ANOVA when the latter’s assumptions have been
met. For between-subjects designs, the factorial test is conservative. Code to execute the routines described in this article
may be downloaded from http://brm.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental. 相似文献