全文获取类型
收费全文 | 804篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
815篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 16篇 |
2018年 | 26篇 |
2017年 | 29篇 |
2016年 | 31篇 |
2015年 | 16篇 |
2014年 | 25篇 |
2013年 | 88篇 |
2012年 | 41篇 |
2011年 | 36篇 |
2010年 | 18篇 |
2009年 | 33篇 |
2008年 | 25篇 |
2007年 | 38篇 |
2006年 | 34篇 |
2005年 | 30篇 |
2004年 | 28篇 |
2003年 | 27篇 |
2002年 | 21篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 15篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 10篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 10篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1965年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有815条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
22.
This paper investigates the role of conditionals in hypothetical reasoning and rational decision making. Its main result is
a proof of a representation theorem for preferences defined on sets of sentences (and, in particular, conditional sentences),
where an agent’s preference for one sentence over another is understood to be a preference for receiving the news conveyed
by the former. The theorem shows that a rational preference ordering of conditional sentences determines probability and desirability
representations of the agent’s degrees of belief and desire that satisfy, in the case of non-conditional sentences, the axioms
of Jeffrey’s decision theory and, in the case of conditional sentences, Adams’ expression for the probabilities of conditionals.
Furthermore, the probability representation is shown to be unique and the desirability representation unique up to positive
linear transformation.
This revised version was published online in June 2006 with corrections to the Cover Date. 相似文献
23.
The accuracy of the two-moment, three-moment, square root, and cube root approximations to the noncentralF distribution was assessed using 7,920 entries from Tiku’s (1967) power tables. Tiku’s tables list exact values of β for α= .005?.05, ν1 = 1?12, ν2 = 2?120, and ?= 0.5?3.0. Analysis of the errors showed generally satisfactory performance for all four approximations. The three-moment approximation was most accurate, registering a maximum error of only .009. The other three approximations had maximum errors of ±.02, except for the square root approximation at ν2 = 2, where maximum errors of .05 occur. Approximation error increased with decreases in ν1 and, less consistently, with increases in ν2. Error was nonmonotonically related to ?. A second investigation explored the accuracy of the approximations at values of αranging from .10 to .90. All four approximations degraded substantially in this situation, with maximum errors ranging from ?.09 to .05. If the analysis is restricted to cases where ν1 τ; 1 and ν2 τ; 2, maximum errors drop to roughly ±.03. We conclude that the approximations perform reasonably well for small αand moderately well for larger values, if certain restrictions are imposed. From a computational standpoint, however, there is little advantage to using approximate as opposed to exact methods unless exact values ofF α are known in advance. 相似文献
24.
Yutong Chen;Leann Wills;Hannah Wise;Bradley T. Erford;Ruonan Yao; 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2024,102(4):406-414
Hazardous or harmful alcohol use is very prevalent among the US population. With 15.1 million adults in the United States currently identified as having an alcohol use disorder, identifying hazardous and harmful drinking earlier is necessary for early intervention strategies. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test's (AUDIT) strong psychometric properties lead it to be a screening tool of choice across countries, cultures, and subgroups. A psychometric analysis aggregates all available evidence of reliability and validity to support the use of an instrument for screening, diagnosis, and treatment planning. AUDIT internal consistency for the 10-item total scale was α = 0.817 (n = 247,963). The two-factor model had the most structure validity support and aggregated convergent validity comparisons with other alcohol use assessments ranged from r = 0.578 to r = 0.701. The optimal cutoff score for diagnostic validity was 6/7, rather than the 8 recommended by the AUDIT authors. Clinically appropriate use and interpretation of the AUDIT as well as study limitations and suggestions for future research were discussed. 相似文献
25.
Mora A. Reinka Bradley Pan-Weisz Elizabeth K. Lawner Diane M. Quinn 《Journal of applied social psychology》2020,50(4):253-261
Large health disparities exist between stigmatized and nonstigmatized groups. In addition to experiencing and anticipating greater discrimination, members of stigmatized groups also tend to demonstrate greater ruminative tendencies in response, which may lead to these poor health outcomes. Even among stigmatized groups, differences in the visibility of stigma lead to different mechanisms through which stigma takes its toll. Previous work has primarily focused on the impact of belonging to a single marginalized group; however, people often belong to multiple marginalized groups, and this likely affects both their health outcomes and their anticipation of stigma. In the current study, we focused on individuals with concealable stigmatized identities (CSIs)—socially stigmatized identities that are not immediately apparent to others—and created a measure of concealable marginalization that captures multiple group memberships. We predicted that those possessing a greater number of CSIs would anticipate more stigma from others, and, in turn, ruminate more about the stigma, which would negatively impact the health. Surveying N = 288 adults with CSIs, we found that possessing a greater number of marginalized concealable identities predicted worse self-reported physical quality of life. These relationships were partially mediated by greater anticipated stigma and brooding rumination in regard to their CSI. This work illuminates a more complete picture of how living with CSIs can take its toll on health. 相似文献
26.
27.
28.
Recent studies have shown that the presentation of concurrent linguistic context can lead to highly efficient performance
in a standard conjunction search task by the induction of an incremental search strategy (Spivey, Tyler, Eberhard, & Tanenhaus,
2001). However, these findings were obtained under anomalously slow speech rate conditions. Accordingly, in the present study,
the effects of concurrent linguistic context on visual search performance were compared when speech was recorded at both a
normal rate and a slow rate. The findings provided clear evidence that the visual search benefit afforded by concurrent linguistic
context was contingent on speech rate, with normal speech producing a smaller benefit. Overall, these findings have important
implications for understanding how linguistic and visual processes interact in real time and suggest a disparity in the temporal
resolution of speech comprehension and visual search processes. 相似文献
29.
Much has been stated in the popular press about the effects of the events of 9/11/01 on employee attitudes about work. This study examined a large sample (N = 70,671) of employees of a multinational manufacturer whose annual employee survey data collection was interrupted by the events. After demonstrating measurement equivalence across time and countries, changes in attitudes pre- and post-9/11 were examined. Only negligible differences were found in Job Satisfaction, Supervisor Evaluation, Stress, and Organizational Commitment to Diversity for U.S. employees or for employees worldwide. Demographic differences in response to events were not found. Implications for understanding effects of stressful external events on employee perceptions of work are discussed. 相似文献
30.
Cohen-Kettenis PT Owen A Kaijser VG Bradley SJ Zucker KJ 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2003,31(1):41-53
This study examined demographic characteristics, social competence, and behavior problems in clinic-referred children with gender identity problems in Toronto, Canada (N = 358), and Utrecht, The Netherlands (N = 130). The Toronto sample was, on average, about a year younger than the Utrecht sample at referral, had a higher percentage of boys, had a higher mean IQ, and was less likely to be living with both parents. On the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), both groups showed, on average, clinical range scores in both social competence and behavior problems. A CBCL-derived measure of poor peer relations showed that boys in both clinics had worse ratings than did the girls. A multiple regression analysis showed that poor peer relations were the strongest predictor of behavior problems in both samples. This study—the first cross-national, cross-clinic comparative analysis of children with gender identity disorder—found far more similarities than differences in both social competence and behavior problems. The most salient demographic difference was age at referral. Cross-national differences in factors that might influence referral patterns are discussed. 相似文献