全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3263篇 |
免费 | 55篇 |
专业分类
3318篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 40篇 |
2020年 | 57篇 |
2019年 | 52篇 |
2018年 | 78篇 |
2017年 | 87篇 |
2016年 | 103篇 |
2015年 | 60篇 |
2014年 | 69篇 |
2013年 | 326篇 |
2012年 | 140篇 |
2011年 | 122篇 |
2010年 | 75篇 |
2009年 | 86篇 |
2008年 | 107篇 |
2007年 | 118篇 |
2006年 | 107篇 |
2005年 | 91篇 |
2004年 | 80篇 |
2003年 | 89篇 |
2002年 | 84篇 |
2001年 | 63篇 |
2000年 | 85篇 |
1999年 | 47篇 |
1998年 | 58篇 |
1997年 | 36篇 |
1996年 | 29篇 |
1995年 | 29篇 |
1994年 | 29篇 |
1993年 | 28篇 |
1992年 | 42篇 |
1991年 | 56篇 |
1990年 | 30篇 |
1989年 | 42篇 |
1988年 | 23篇 |
1987年 | 29篇 |
1986年 | 22篇 |
1985年 | 38篇 |
1984年 | 28篇 |
1982年 | 30篇 |
1981年 | 41篇 |
1980年 | 34篇 |
1979年 | 30篇 |
1978年 | 32篇 |
1977年 | 24篇 |
1975年 | 36篇 |
1974年 | 36篇 |
1973年 | 28篇 |
1972年 | 23篇 |
1971年 | 22篇 |
1967年 | 21篇 |
排序方式: 共有3318条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
Wayne Viney Neil Johnson Larry Jacobson 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1978,30(4):747-754
Sixty-four male Wistar rats were given acquisition training in an enclosed straight-alley runway which could be adjusted for angles of inclination. The 2×2×2 design involved two angles of inclination in acquisition (0° and 40°) and two angles of inclination in extinction (0° and 40°). Between acquisition and extinction, half the subjects were exposed to a latent extinction procedure and half served as controls. Number of responses in a 30-min extinction session was an inverse function of effort required in extinction. Additionally, latent extinction procedures resulted in reduced resistance to extinction, but only when the effort conditions of acquisition and extinction were constant. When the effort conditions of acquisition and extinction were dissimilar, latent extinction procedures resulted in increased resistance to extinction. The results raise questions about the nature of the learning which occurs during latent extinction training. 相似文献
42.
43.
Results of 1,579 observations of cars entering or exiting campus parking lots showed direct relationships between seat belt wearing and the intrusiveness of the engineering device designed to induce belt usage, and between device intrusiveness and system defeat. For example, all drivers with working interlocks or unlimited buzzer reminders were wearing a seat belt; but 62% of the systems with interlocks or unlimited buzzers had been defeated, and only 15.9% of the drivers in these cars were wearing a seat belt. The normative data indicated marked ineffectiveness of the negative reinforcement contingencies implied by current seat belt inducement systems; but suggested that unlimited buzzer systems would be the optimal system currently available if contingencies were developed to discourage the disconnection and circumvention of such systems. Positive reinforcement strategies are discussed that would be quite feasible for large-scale promotion of seat belt usage. 相似文献
44.
Norbert Johnson 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1980,59(1):49-51
The author believes that some counselors may avoid the use of Rational Emotive Therapy techniques because they feel that they could not deal with clients in the same active-directive manner as of Albert Ellis, the originator of RET. Counselors who may be interested in applying RET principles with clients are urged to adapt the techniques of RET to their own personalities and styles of counseling. 相似文献
45.
Gregory R. Lockhead Robert C. Johnson Fran M. Gold 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1980,27(6):545-549
At least three localization illusions can be produced by varying the time relations between two stimuli. Two of these, phi motion and Békésy’s illusion, have been induced in areas lacking receptors. To learn if this also occurred with the third illusion, sensory saltation, people reported their observations when Geldard’s visual rabbit was induced by stimulating points around the optic disk. The rabbit crossed the blind spot as well as other portions of the eyes, and the illusory point was often localized within the region of the blind spot. All three illusions can be localized in an area devoid of receptors. 相似文献
46.
Two major features of the overcorrection procedure, restitution and positive practice, were analyzed for their educative and suppressive properties in the treatment of profoundly retarded adults. Positive practice techniques that were topographically similar and dissimilar to the target behavior were studied. Eating behavior and puzzle performance were observed. Restitutional overcorrection and both forms of positive practice were effective for suppressing inappropriate behaviors. Even when appropriate behaviors had been acquired by positive practice, restitution and dissimilar positive practice were generally ineffective for increasing their rate of occurrence. However, topographically similar positive practice was successful as a means of teaching new appropriate behaviors. 相似文献
47.
This paper is a reply to an article in this journal by M. Zimmerman titled “The Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment: A Comment on Elardo and Bradley's Review”. We found Zimmerman's critique of our original review article (Developmental Review, 1981, 1, 113–145) to contain points which were well taken, and several which seemed pedantic. Specifically, we address his concerns about our discussion of the HOME scale's interrater reliability, test-retest stability, concurrent validity, and predictive validity; and we reply to his comments about the uses of the HOME scale for purposes of screening and matching environments. 相似文献
48.
The present research conceptualizes life structure, or the pattern of life, as the ordering of priorities among competing alternatives. Intentions to emphasize career, marriage, or both were investigated for college students. Variations in these intentions were seen as resulting from differences in two classes of variables: (a) the perceived consequences of emphasizing the alternative; and (b) perceived normative pressures. A subjective probability model of the relationship between beliefs and behavioral intentions was used as a framework for determining the importance of these factors. The considerations identified as most likely to have a direct impact on life structure dealt primarily with interpersonal relations, particularly marriage and family. In general, the data suggested that the subjective probability model may be useful for further understanding of life structure decisions. 相似文献
49.
Robert O Hansson Robert Hogan John A Johnson David Schroeder 《Journal of research in personality》1983,17(2):186-197
This study identified stable components of Type A behavior as usually defined, and examined their relationships with more traditional psychological constructs. Overall scores on the most popular Type A measure (a version of the Jenkins Activity Survey) were moderately associated with ambition but largely unrelated to measures of adjustment. An item analysis yielded subcomponents of Drivenness and Involvement. Drivenness was unrelated to ambition, but was positively related to anxiety and negatively related to interpersonal perceptiveness. Involvement was positively related to ambition and to interpersonal competence. It is suggested that the Jenkins Activity Survey measures a heterogeneous mixture of constructive commitment and self-defeating responses that clouds its psychological interpretation. 相似文献
50.
Kenneth J. Zucker Jo -Anne K. Finegan Robert W. Doering Susan J. Bradley 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1983,11(2):287-298
The Draw-a-Person test was used to evaluate a number of questions regarding children referred for potential problems in their gender-identity development (N = 36). Sibling (N = 31), psychiatric (N = 23), and normal (N = 30) children served as comparison groups. The major results included the following: (1) The gender-referred children were more likely to draw an opposite-sex person when requested to "draw a person" than were the other three groups; (2) the gender-referred children who drew an opposite-sex person were more likely to play with opposite-sex toys and dress-up apparel on a free-play task than were the gender-referred children who drew a same-sex person; (3) the gender-referred children drew taller opposite-sex persons than same-sex persons; (4) using Koppitz's (1968) criteria, the normal children had a smaller proportion of emotional disturbance indicators in their same-sex drawings than did the other three groups. These findings were discussed with regard to psychometric and interpretive issues in the assessment of children with atypical gender-role behavior. 相似文献