全文获取类型
收费全文 | 801篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
812篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 16篇 |
2018年 | 26篇 |
2017年 | 29篇 |
2016年 | 31篇 |
2015年 | 16篇 |
2014年 | 25篇 |
2013年 | 88篇 |
2012年 | 41篇 |
2011年 | 35篇 |
2010年 | 18篇 |
2009年 | 33篇 |
2008年 | 25篇 |
2007年 | 38篇 |
2006年 | 34篇 |
2005年 | 30篇 |
2004年 | 28篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 21篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 15篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 10篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1965年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有812条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
751.
John Towler Angela Gosling Bradley Duchaine Martin Eimer 《Journal of Neuropsychology》2016,10(1):15-32
Individuals with developmental prosopagnosia (DP) have a severe difficulty recognizing the faces of known individuals in the absence of any history of neurological damage. These recognition problems may be linked to selective deficits in the holistic/configural processing of faces. We used two‐tone Mooney images to study the processing of faces versus non‐face objects in DP when it is based on holistic information (or the facial gestalt) in the absence of obvious local cues about facial features. A rapid adaptation procedure was employed for a group of 16 DPs. Naturalistic photographs of upright faces were preceded by upright or inverted Mooney faces or by Mooney houses. DPs showed face‐sensitive N170 components in response to Mooney faces versus houses, and N170 amplitude reductions for inverted as compared to upright Mooney faces. They also showed the typical pattern of N170 adaptation effects, with reduced N170 components when upright naturalistic test faces were preceded by upright Mooney faces, demonstrating that the perception of Mooney and naturalistic faces recruits shared neural populations. Our findings demonstrate that individuals with DP can utilize global information about face configurations for categorical discriminations between faces and non‐face objects, and suggest that face processing deficits emerge primarily at more fine‐grained higher level stages of face perception. 相似文献
752.
Gerta Bardhoshi Kelly Duncan Bradley T. Erford 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2016,94(3):356-373
This meta‐analysis reviewed 192 scholarly works from 1993 to 2013 using the Beck Anxiety Inventory (Beck & Steer, 1993 ). Aggregated internal consistency (coefficient alpha) was .91 (k = 117), and test–retest reliability was .65 (k = 18). Convergent comparisons were robust across 33 different anxiety instruments and the Beck Depression Inventory–II (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996 ). Structural validity primarily supported the original 2‐factor solution proposed by Beck and Steer ( 1993 ), and diagnostic accuracy varied according to the sample size and criterion cutoff score. 相似文献
753.
Christine M. Steeger Dawn M. Gondoli Bradley S. Gibson Rebecca A. Morrissey 《Child neuropsychology》2016,22(4):394-419
This study examined the individual and combined effects of two nonpharmacological treatments for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Cogmed working memory training (CWMT) for adolescents and behavioral parent training (BPT) for mothers. Ninety-one adolescents (ages 11–15) and their mothers were randomized to one of four CWMT and BPT treatment and active control (placebo) group combinations of 5-week interventions. At pre- and posttest, mothers and teachers completed rating forms, and adolescents completed neuropsychological measures of working memory (WM). Individual intervention effects showed that treatment CWMT significantly improved WM spans, whereas there were no significant differences for treatment or control BPT on reports of parent-related outcomes. Combined treatment effects indicated an overall pattern of greatest improvements for the control CWMT/treatment BPT group, as compared to the other three groups, on adolescent WM deficit, behavioral regulation problems, and global executive deficit. Most significant effects for outcomes were main effects of improvements over time. A combination of CWMT and BPT did not result in increased treatment gains. However, potential effects of combined treatment may have been masked by greater perceived benefits arising from lack of struggle in the nonadaptive, CWMT active control condition. Future combined intervention research should focus on specific, theoretically driven WM deficits among individuals with ADHD, should include possible adaptations to the standard CWMT program, should examine effectiveness of cognitive treatments combined with contextual interventions and should utilize appropriate control groups to fully understand the unique and combined effects of interventions. 相似文献
754.
Bradley Armour‐Garb 《Philosophy Compass》2012,7(4):256-266
In this paper, I address some of the chief challenges, or problems, for Deflationary Theories of Truth, viz., the Generalization Problem, the Conservativeness Argument, and the Success Argument. 相似文献
755.
756.
This study examines whether top management team (TMT) personality and leadership are associated with organizational effectiveness beyond the effects of CEO personality and leadership, as suggested by upper echelons theory. Using direct measures of personality and leadership, rather than proxy variables from archival sources or demographic data, we found that mean levels of conscientiousness among TMT members were related to lagged indicators of organizational performance, as were CEO conscientiousness and transformational leadership. Follower commitment to the organization was found to be associated with higher levels of transformational leadership from both the CEO and TMT. The results are consistent with the upper echelons perspective that organizational effectiveness is influenced not only by the CEO but also by a dominant coalition of leaders. Yet, the results also show that the CEO plays a distinct role in influencing organizational financial performance and collective organizational commitment. Theoretical and practical implications of these results are discussed. 相似文献
757.
We reanalyzed the behavioral and fMRI data from seven previously published studies of working memory in order to assess the
behavioral and neural effects of item-nonspecific proactive interference (PI; attributable to the accrual of antecedent information
independent of the repetition of particular items). We hypothesized that item-nonspecific PI, implicated in age-related declines
in working memory performance, is mediated by the same mechanism(s) that mediate item-specific PI (occurring when an invalid
memory probe matches a memorandum from the previous trial). Reaction time increased across trials as a function of position
within the block, a trend that reversed across the duration of each multiblock experiment. The fMRI analyses revealed sensitivity
to item-nonspecific PI during the probe epoch in the left anterior inferior frontal gyrus and the left dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex (PFC). They also revealed a negative trend, across trials, in the transient probe-evoked component of the global signal.
A common PFC-based mechanism may mediate many forms of PI. 相似文献
758.
Jones M Love BC Maddox WT 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2006,32(2):316-332
Accounts of learning and generalization typically focus on factors related to lasting changes in representation (i.e., long-term memory). The authors present evidence that shorter term effects also play a critical role in determining performance and that these recency effects can be subdivided into perceptual and decisional components. Experimental results based on a probabilistic category structure show that the previous stimulus exerts a contrastive effect on the current percept (perceptual recency) and that responses are biased toward or away from the previous feedback, depending on the similarity between successive stimuli (decisional recency). A method for assessing these recency effects is presented that clarifies open questions regarding stimulus generalization and perceptual contrast effects in categorization and in other domains. 相似文献
759.
Sara?King Daniel?A.?WaschbuschEmail author Bradley?W.?Frankland Brendan?F.?Andrade Catherine?M.?Thurston Lisa?McNutt Branka?Terrio Northern Partners in Action for Child Youth Services 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2005,27(2):77-88
Cluster analysis was used to investigate the classification of comorbid Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) +Conduct Problems (CP). Teachers rated 1394 elementary school children on symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, opposition, and conduct problems using the ADS-IV behavior-rating scale. Results suggested that a seven-cluster solution, including two distinct comorbid clusters, was the most appropriate method of classifying children according to behavioral symptoms. A one-way MANOVA showed that overall cluster profiles differed significantly from each other, whereas one-way ANOVAs indicated that clusters differed on age and symptom scores. Chi-square analyses indicated that clusters differed on gender. Post hoc analyses and effect sizes calculations revealed that the comorbid clusters differed from each other and from their single-disorder counterparts on some variables, but not on all. These results provide support for an additive conceptualisation of ADHD +CP. 相似文献
760.
Structural contingency theory and individual differences: examination of external and internal person-team fit 总被引:10,自引:0,他引:10
Hollenbeck JR Moon H Ellis AP West BJ Ilgen DR Sheppard L Porter CO Wagner JA 《The Journal of applied psychology》2002,87(3):599-606
This article develops and tests a structurally based, integrated theory of person-team fit. The theory developed is an extension of structural contingency theory and considers issues of external fit simultaneously with its examination of internal fit at the team level. Results from 80 teams working on an interdependent team task indicate that divisional structures demand high levels of cognitive ability on the part of teammembers. However, the advantages of high cognitive ability in divisional structures are neutralized when there is poor external fit between the structure and the environment. Instead, emotional stability becomes a critical factor among teammembers when a divisional structure is out of alignment with its environment. Individual differences seem to play little or no role in functional structures, regardless of the degree of external fit. 相似文献