全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1613篇 |
免费 | 26篇 |
专业分类
1639篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 20篇 |
2020年 | 27篇 |
2019年 | 26篇 |
2018年 | 40篇 |
2017年 | 44篇 |
2016年 | 49篇 |
2015年 | 32篇 |
2014年 | 40篇 |
2013年 | 195篇 |
2012年 | 67篇 |
2011年 | 65篇 |
2010年 | 35篇 |
2009年 | 49篇 |
2008年 | 58篇 |
2007年 | 63篇 |
2006年 | 55篇 |
2005年 | 54篇 |
2004年 | 47篇 |
2003年 | 48篇 |
2002年 | 41篇 |
2001年 | 21篇 |
2000年 | 30篇 |
1999年 | 20篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 19篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 18篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 15篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1985年 | 21篇 |
1984年 | 17篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 16篇 |
1980年 | 13篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 28篇 |
1977年 | 22篇 |
1976年 | 11篇 |
1975年 | 14篇 |
1974年 | 15篇 |
1973年 | 14篇 |
1968年 | 9篇 |
1967年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有1639条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
141.
142.
Remembering pictures: pleasure and arousal in memory. 总被引:17,自引:0,他引:17
M M Bradley M K Greenwald M C Petry P J Lang 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》1992,18(2):379-390
Incidental memory performance for pictures that varied along the affective dimensions of pleasantness and arousal was assessed. For both an immediate and delayed (1 year later) free-recall task, only the arousal dimension had a stable effect on memory performance: Pictures rated as highly arousing were remembered better than low-arousal stimuli. This effect was corroborated in a speeded recognition test, in which high-arousal materials encoded earlier in the experiment produced faster reaction times than their low-arousal counterparts. Pleasantness affected reaction time decisions only for pictures not encoded earlier. These results suggest that whereas both the dimensions of pleasantness and arousal are processed at initial encoding, long-term memory performance is mainly affected by arousal. 相似文献
143.
A MOTIVATIONAL ANALYSIS OF EMOTION: Reflex-Cortex Connections 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
144.
The statistical simulation program DATASIM is designed to conduct large-scale sampling experiments on microcomputers. Monte Carlo procedures are used to investigate the Type I and Type II error rates for statistical tests when one or more assumptions are systematically violated-assumptions, for example, regarding normality, homogeneity of variance or covariance, mini-mum expected cell frequencies, and the like. In the present paper, we report several initial tests of the data-generating algorithms employed by DATASIM. The results indicate that the uniform and standard normal deviate generators perform satisfactorily. Furthermore, Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests show that the sampling distributions ofz, t, F, χ2, andr generated by DATASIM simulations follow the appropriate theoretical distributions. Finally, estimates of Type I error rates obtained by DATASIM under various patterns of violations of assumptions are in close agreement with the results of previous analytical and empirical studies; These converging lines of evidence suggest that DATASIM may well prove to be a reliable and productive tool for conducting statistical simulation research. 相似文献
145.
146.
Karin Mogg Brendan P. Bradley Nina Hallowell 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1994,47(4):841-864
Attentional biases for threat stimuli were assessed in high and low trait anxious subjects (n = 66) using a probe detection task. To examine the effects of trait anxiety and situational stressors, each subject was tested three times: Under no stress, laboratory-induced stress, and examination-induced stress. To evaluate the role of awareness, half the word stimuli were presented very briefly (14 msec) and masked, and the other half were presented for 500 msec without a mask. Results showed that high trait anxious subjects under exam stress showed an attentional bias towards unmasked threat stimuli compared with low trait subjects. This effect was not found under lab-induced stress, suggesting that the attentional bias for unmasked threat in high trait subjects may be a function of a prolonged stressor, rather than a transient increase in state anxiety. The results from the masked exposure condition were not predicted; high trait anxious subjects shifted attention towards the spatial location of threat words despite lack of awareness of their lexical content, but this bias was only apparent in the no-stress condition. The results are discussed in relation to recent cognitive theories of anxiety. 相似文献
147.
This study examined whether patients with Korsakoff's disease suffer from increased PI during encoding. The ability of the name of one category, e.g., BIRD, to prime the processing of members of another category, e.g., BODY PARTS, in a lexical decision task was used to assess the amount of PI during encoding. This task required a subject to inhibit the normal associations to BIRD. Young normals (25 years), older normals (48 years), alcoholics (45 years), and alcoholic Korsakoff patients (59 years) performed two lexical decision tasks. In the first experiment, the appearance of the neutral prime XXX 750 msec before the probe signaled that if the probe was a word, there was a 75% chance that it was from a particular category (e.g., BODY PARTS). The prime facilitated reaction time for words from the expected category for all four groups. The prime slowed reaction time for words that were not from the expected category for the young normals but did not influence reaction time for unexpected words for the three older groups. The second experiment was identical to the first except that a category word was used as the prime. The category word used as the prime was unrelated to the category of the words that were likely to follow it. For example, BIRD might be used to signal the likelihood that the word would be from the category, BODY PARTS. Again, young normals were slower to respond to unexpected probe words, but the three older groups were not. Again, the prime facilitated reaction time for expected words for the young normals, older normals, and alcoholics. However, the word prime did not facilitate reaction time for expected words for the alcoholic Korsakoff patients. That the word prime did not facilitate reaction time for the Korsakoff patients was viewed as evidence that they were unable to inhibit its normal associations and were more sensitive to PI from these associations than the other subjects. 相似文献
148.
A semiautomatic version of the Wisconsin General Test Apparatus is described along with circuitry. Comparison data are provided for the semiautomatic and manual WGTA obtained under comparable procedures and conditions. Response latencies are given for go/no-go object discrimination problems and subsequent retention tests. 相似文献
149.
Visual salience and perceptual load may both influence the efficiency of visual selection. Recently, Gibson and Bryant (2008) showed that perceptual load can dominate color salience in a distractor interference paradigm. The purpose of the present study was to investigate the possibility that knowledge (of color or load) may modulate the relative operation of these two mechanisms. Consistent with previous findings, perceptual load dominated color salience, but only in certain contexts in which display load was mixed and high-load displays preceded other high-load displays. More important, color salience dominated perceptual load in other contexts in which display load was mixed and low-load displays preceded high-load displays. In addition, color salience also dominated perceptual load in contexts in which display load was fixed and advance knowledge of load was available. Altogether, the present findings suggest that the competition between color salience and perceptual load can vary as a function of task context, thereby supporting top-down accounts, although the precise aspect of task context remains to be identified. 相似文献
150.
Alexander Arnold 《Australasian journal of philosophy》2013,91(1):165-172
According to some philosophers who accept a propositional conception of evidence, someone's evidence includes a proposition only if it is true. I argue against this thesis by appealing to the possibility of knowledge from falsehood. 相似文献