全文获取类型
收费全文 | 180篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
184篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 7篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 5篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 6篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 17篇 |
2013年 | 16篇 |
2012年 | 3篇 |
2011年 | 3篇 |
2010年 | 4篇 |
2009年 | 4篇 |
2008年 | 2篇 |
2007年 | 11篇 |
2006年 | 4篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 5篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 2篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1965年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有184条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
161.
It's more than skin‐deep: The relationship between social victimization and telomere length in adolescence
下载免费PDF全文

Maria E. Guarneri‐White Allyson A. Arana Erin Q. Boyd Lauri A. Jensen‐Campbell 《Aggressive behavior》2018,44(4):337-347
162.
Providing explicit information disrupts implicit motor learning after basal ganglia stroke
下载免费PDF全文

Despite their purported neuroanatomic and functional isolation, empirical evidence suggests that sometimes conscious explicit processes can influence implicit motor skill learning. Our goal was to determine if the provision of explicit information affected implicit motor-sequence learning after damage to the basal ganglia. Individuals with stroke affecting the basal ganglia (BG) and healthy controls (HC) practiced a continuous implicit motor-sequencing task; half were provided with explicit information (EI) and half were not (No-EI). The focus of brain damage for both BG groups was in the putamen. All of the EI participants were at least explicitly aware of the repeating sequence. Across three days of practice, explicit information had a differential effect on the groups. Explicit information disrupted acquisition performance in participants with basal ganglia stroke but not healthy controls. By retention (day 4), a dissociation was apparent--explicit information hindered implicit learning in participants with basal ganglia lesions but aided healthy controls. It appears that after basal ganglia stroke explicit information is less helpful in the development of the motor plan than is discovering a motor solution using the implicit system alone. This may be due to the increased demand placed on working memory by explicit information. Thus, basal ganglia integrity may be a crucial factor in determining the efficacy of explicit information for implicit motor-sequence learning. 相似文献
163.
Boyd Millar 《Synthese》2010,174(3):445-461
In Sense and Content, Christopher Peacocke points out that two equally-sized trees at different distances from the perceiver are normally represented
to be the same size, despite the fact that in a certain sense the nearer tree looks bigger; he concludes on the basis of this observation that visual experiences possess irreducibly phenomenal properties. This argument
has received the most attention of all of Peacocke’s arguments for separatism—the view that the intentional and phenomenal
properties of experiences are independent of one another. However, despite its notoriety, the argument is widely misunderstood
and underappreciated. I argue that once the structure of the argument is clarified and the replies that have been offered
are considered closely, one must conclude that the trees argument is successful. 相似文献
164.
165.
Carolyn M. Boyd Arnold B. Bakker Silvia Pignata Anthony H. Winefield Nicole Gillespie Con Stough 《Psychologie appliquee》2011,60(1):112-140
A longitudinal test of the Job Demands‐Resources (JD‐R) model of work stress and engagement ( Bakker & Demerouti, 2007 ; Demerouti et al., 2001 ) was conducted in a sample of Australian university academics (N= 296). The aim was to extend the JD‐R model by (1) determining how well job demands (work pressure, academic workload) and job resources (procedural fairness, job autonomy) would predict psychological strain and organisational commitment over a three‐year period, and (2) incorporating longitudinal tests of reversed causation. The results of SEM analyses showed that Time 1 resources directly predicted Time 2 strain and organisational commitment, but that Time 1 demands predicted Time 2 strain only indirectly via job resources. We did not find evidence for reversed causation. We discuss possible mediators of the relationships between working conditions and work stress outcomes, and the practical implications of the results. 相似文献
166.
Olagoke Ayokunle A. Floyd Brenikki Caskey Rachel Hebert-Beirne Jennifer Boyd Andrew D. Molina Yamile 《Journal of religion and health》2022,61(2):1734-1749
Journal of Religion and Health - Religion is a complex and sociocultural driver of human papillomavirus (HPV) vaccination decisions, but its exact role has been mixed/unclear. We used a... 相似文献
167.
Webb NB 《International journal of group psychotherapy》2005,55(3):355-374
The article reviews the use of debriefing and support groups for children who experienced the deaths of family members in the traumatic events of September 11, 2001. An overview of children's normal age-related responses to bereavement and of the special circumstances of trauma and grief provides the foundation for understanding the unique family and community stressors of 9/11 that impacted the children and group leaders. Stress-reduction exercises suitable for use with traumatized children in bereavement groups are presented together with specific drawing and written activities to encourage the processing of feelings. 相似文献
168.
This study investigated the merits of substituting the Digit Span subtest for an invalid Verbal Scale subtest versus a proration method in calculating WISC--R Verbal and Full Scale IQ. Subjects were 93 child and adolescent psychiatric patients (67 boys and 26 girls) who ranged in age from 8 to 16 yr. Analysis indicated that the use of Digit Span as a substitute for the regularly administered Verbal subtests was inferior to the use of the comparable proration method. Although relatively few cases of misclassification occurred for either method in relation to Full Scale IQ, the rate of Verbal IQ misclassification by the Digit Span substitution method was significantly greater than with use of its proration. When faced with a choice, clinicians should attach greater validity to prorated estimates of a child's WISC--R Verbal IQ. 相似文献
169.
We investigate M-trees, that is, trees with structure possible at each node or level. M is a mathematical structure such as a set or a Cartesian product. An extension of Pólya's theorem is proved which allows the number of M-trees for a given number of nodes to be counted. The special case of componential trees is investigated. Here M is a Cartesian product of 0's and 1's. A componential analysis is a componential tree of depth 1, that is, there is no hierarchy. We prove that for any componential tree there exists a componential analysis which makes the same predictions on a triad test of judged similarity. A brief empirical example is given, in which a componential tree is applied as a model of a sernantic domain. 相似文献
170.