全文获取类型
收费全文 | 16095篇 |
免费 | 619篇 |
国内免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
16718篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 213篇 |
2018年 | 258篇 |
2017年 | 219篇 |
2016年 | 257篇 |
2015年 | 169篇 |
2014年 | 261篇 |
2013年 | 1272篇 |
2012年 | 432篇 |
2011年 | 485篇 |
2010年 | 289篇 |
2009年 | 312篇 |
2008年 | 413篇 |
2007年 | 442篇 |
2006年 | 421篇 |
2005年 | 343篇 |
2004年 | 372篇 |
2003年 | 334篇 |
2002年 | 329篇 |
2001年 | 451篇 |
2000年 | 510篇 |
1999年 | 342篇 |
1998年 | 205篇 |
1997年 | 154篇 |
1996年 | 166篇 |
1995年 | 164篇 |
1992年 | 276篇 |
1991年 | 285篇 |
1990年 | 296篇 |
1989年 | 276篇 |
1988年 | 267篇 |
1987年 | 234篇 |
1986年 | 246篇 |
1985年 | 259篇 |
1984年 | 191篇 |
1983年 | 207篇 |
1982年 | 158篇 |
1980年 | 156篇 |
1979年 | 248篇 |
1978年 | 216篇 |
1977年 | 150篇 |
1975年 | 215篇 |
1974年 | 242篇 |
1973年 | 268篇 |
1972年 | 225篇 |
1971年 | 210篇 |
1970年 | 162篇 |
1969年 | 195篇 |
1968年 | 248篇 |
1967年 | 239篇 |
1966年 | 209篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
32.
33.
F. Diane Barth 《American journal of psychoanalysis》1988,48(3):198-210
It is the thesis of this paper that although analysts have always recognized that narcissistic injury may trigger envious feelings, the significance of self-esteem as both a motivator and response to envious feelings has not yet been sufficiently explored. Traditional drive-defense or object instinctual explanations tend to diminish awareness of the importance of self-esteem in the experience of envy. The focus on drives or repetition of early patterns of object relations does not always take into account the significance of the cycle in which damaged self-esteem leads to envy, the component parts of which may cause further damage to self-esteem, leading to more envy, and so on. I am suggesting that it is often an attempt to avoid painful injury to one's self-esteem, as well as the related attempt to maintain a positively colored sense of self, and not a repression of drives or a repetition of some aspect of early object relationships which must be understood in order to fully comprehend both feelings of envy and the need to keep such feelings out of conscious awareness. 相似文献
34.
As part of a large survey of addictive behavior in high school students, 43% of a sample of 278 (26% of the males, 57% of the females) scored above the cutoff point set by Overeaters Anonymous on their scale for assessing compulsive overeating. While this at-risk group did not report poorer general adjustment, health, or school achievement than did the students not at risk, they did significantly more often perceive their life quality and relationship with the person closest to them as less positive. The at-risk subsample indicated the defensive effectiveness of overeating in their significantly more frequent report of dissociative experiences while eating, and less severe ratings of insecurity, worrying, and daydreaming. One of the most salient findings was the at-risk students' more frequent report of addictive problems in their parents (overeating, alcohol and drug use, and gambling). 相似文献
35.
Four clinical examples of oedipal-based transference across gender lines are presented with the aim of illustrating (1) its existence, (2) the defenses against its emergence, and (3) the use of the analyst's gender as both an organizer of and resistance to certain transference manifestations. Factors that contribute to the availability for analysis of cross-gender transference are discussed, as are the resistances and other obstacles to its actualization. 相似文献
36.
37.
Traits and states are concepts that people use to both describe and understand themselves and others. We show that people view these concepts as prototype-based categories that have a graded internal structure and fuzzy boundaries and identify a set of attributes that define the prototypical cores of two categories: traits and states. Prototypical traits are stable, long-lasting, and internally caused. Prototypical states are temporary, brief, and caused by external circumstances. These prototypes are not defined by averages, as the family-resemblance principle would suggest, but by ideal (or extreme) attribute values. Like other ideal-based categories, traits and states serve particular goals. Trait concepts permit people to predict the present from the past; state concepts identify those behaviors that can be controlled by manipulating the situation. These two complementary schemas are part of the extensive theory of psychological causality that is implicit in language. Abstract social concepts differ from object categories in their category standards, the nature of their attributes, and the interrelations among those attributes. 相似文献
38.
A D Pick C F Palmer B L Hennessy M G Unze R K Jones R M Richardson 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1988,45(1):28-51
Children's perception of scale and contour in melodies was investigated in five studies. Experimental tasks included judging transposed renditions of melodies (Studies 1 and 3), discriminating between transposed renditions of a melody (Study 2), judging contour-preserving transformations of melodies (Study 4), and judging similarity to a familiar target melody of transformations preserving rhythm or rhythm and contour (Study 5). The first and second studies showed that young children detect key transposition changes even in familiar melodies and they perceive similarity over key transpositions even in unfamiliar melodies. Young children also are sensitive to melodic contour over transformations that preserve it (Study 5), yet they distinguish spontaneously between melodies with the same contour and different intervals (Study 4). The key distance effect reported in the literature did not occur in the tasks of this investigation (Studies 1 and 3), and it may be apparent only for melodies shorter or more impoverished than those used here. 相似文献
39.
F. Charles Mace Michael L. Hock Joseph S. Lalli Barbara J. West Phillip Belfiore Elizabeth Pinter D. Kirby Brown 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1988,21(2):123-141
Behavioral momentum refers to the tendency for behavior to persist following a change in environmental conditions. The greater the rate of reinforcement, the greater the behavioral momentum. The intervention for noncompliance consisted of issuing a sequence of commands with which the subject was very likely to comply (i.e., high-probability commands) immediately prior to issuing a low-probability command. In each of five experiments, the high-probability command sequence resulted in a “momentum” of compliant responding that persisted when a low-probability request was issued. Results showed the antecedent high-probability command sequence increased compliance and decreased compliance latency and task duration. “Momentum-like” effects were shown to be distinct from experimenter attention and to depend on the contiguity between the high-probability command sequence and the low-probability command. 相似文献
40.