首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   3939篇
  免费   1421篇
  国内免费   82篇
  2023年   12篇
  2022年   6篇
  2021年   86篇
  2020年   98篇
  2019年   395篇
  2018年   294篇
  2017年   399篇
  2016年   391篇
  2015年   370篇
  2014年   317篇
  2013年   562篇
  2012年   310篇
  2011年   226篇
  2010年   269篇
  2009年   238篇
  2008年   236篇
  2007年   165篇
  2006年   135篇
  2005年   153篇
  2004年   109篇
  2003年   114篇
  2002年   135篇
  2001年   94篇
  2000年   78篇
  1999年   49篇
  1998年   20篇
  1997年   15篇
  1996年   10篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   8篇
  1991年   10篇
  1990年   13篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   8篇
  1986年   10篇
  1985年   6篇
  1984年   9篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   6篇
  1981年   6篇
  1980年   4篇
  1978年   6篇
  1977年   6篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   6篇
  1973年   6篇
  1968年   3篇
排序方式: 共有5442条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
How do speakers design what they say in order to communicate effectively with groups of addressees who vary in their background knowledge of the topic at hand? Prior findings indicate that when a speaker addresses a pair of listeners with discrepant knowledge, that speakers Aim Low, designing their utterances for the least knowledgeable of the two addressees. Here, we test the hypothesis that speakers will depart from an Aim Low approach in order to efficiently communicate with larger groups of interacting partners. Further, we ask whether the cognitive demands of tracking multiple conversational partners' perspectives places limitations on successful audience design. We find that speakers can successfully track information about what up to four of their partners do and do not know in conversation. When addressing groups of 3–4 addressees at once, speakers design language based on the combined knowledge of the group. These findings point to an audience design process that simultaneously represents the perspectives of multiple other individuals and combines these representations in order to design utterances that strike a balance between the different needs of the individuals within the group.  相似文献   
12.
Ralph Bedell, professor emeritus of educational and counseling psychology, was interviewed to gain insight into the influence of the National Defense Counseling and Guidance Institutes on the history of counseling and to learn about the man who was responsible for the NDEA program.  相似文献   
13.
14.
15.
16.
School psychologists serving a national, systematic sample of public elementary and secondary schools were surveyed regarding recent referrals. Most referrals were initiated by school staff members (75% of referrals), and the majority occured initially through formal mechanisms (55% of referrals). Most informal requests for assistance or information eventually became, or were thought likely to become, formal referrals. In 92% of cases, the referral agent's initial communication with the psychologist included some designation of what the pupil was doing or not doing that was viewed as problematic. Poor academic performance was of primary concern in 52% of referrals and social/emotional problems in 31%. More boys were referred than girls, and one-half of referrals from the elementary grades involved pupils in grades K-2. Frequency of referral and reason for referral were not independent of pupil's grade or sex. Reason for referral was independent of time of year (October vs. May) and geographical region.  相似文献   
17.
It has been suggested that the systematic decline of partial report as the delay of the partial-report cue increases is due to a time-related loss of location information. Moreover, the backward masking effect is said to be precipitated by the disruption of location information before and after identification. Results from three experiments do not support these claims when new indices of location information and of item information are used. Instead, it was found that the systematic decline in partial report was due to a time-related loss of item information, and location information was affected neither by the delay of the partial-report cue nor by the delay of backward masking. Subjects adopted the "select-then-identify" mode of processing.  相似文献   
18.
Saline-treated and amphetamine-treated (7 mg/kg, ip, immediate) male rats from a Sprague-Dawley substrain were observed in two test environments designed to elicit different investigative responses in normal rats. Snout contact with the substrate was generated by placing the rat in a small enclosed cage. Absence of snout contact was induced by placement of the rat on a square elevated platform. Detailed ethological records were kept of locomotion, rearing, sitting, grooming, gnawing, and sleeping throughout the 90-min session. Amphetamine-treated rats incorporated environmentally contingent bodily postures into their forms of stereotyped behavior. The postures were characteristic of those evinced initially by the saline-treated rats in the same test environment. The control rats showed appropriate changes in their investigative behavior when the apparatus was changed at 10 and at 30 min postinjection. The amphetamine-treated rats, however, were completely unresponsive to such changes at 30 min and only partially so at 10 min postinjection. It was concluded that there is a temporal gradient of decreasing readiness to modify repetitive behavior after a single, large dose of amphetamine.  相似文献   
19.
20.
速示条件下辨认汉字与无意义图形的实验研究   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
尹文刚 《心理学报》1984,17(3):55-61
本实验是对60名中国大学生进行的辨认汉字与无意义图形的速示研究。结果表明,无论是右利还是左利,多数人在辨认中显示出两脑均势的现象。其中汉字辨认无视野差异的占总实验人数的60%,无意义图形辨认无视野差异的占56.66%。这与用表音字对欧美人进行的研究不同,它意味着中国人在认知过程中可能具有特殊的方式。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号