首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1814篇
  免费   60篇
  1874篇
  2021年   29篇
  2020年   21篇
  2019年   30篇
  2018年   40篇
  2017年   41篇
  2016年   57篇
  2015年   40篇
  2014年   41篇
  2013年   192篇
  2012年   66篇
  2011年   99篇
  2010年   44篇
  2009年   42篇
  2008年   69篇
  2007年   70篇
  2006年   56篇
  2005年   54篇
  2004年   70篇
  2003年   47篇
  2002年   52篇
  2001年   36篇
  2000年   30篇
  1999年   25篇
  1998年   30篇
  1997年   20篇
  1996年   20篇
  1995年   25篇
  1994年   23篇
  1993年   17篇
  1992年   25篇
  1991年   18篇
  1990年   27篇
  1989年   17篇
  1988年   26篇
  1987年   18篇
  1985年   17篇
  1984年   18篇
  1983年   23篇
  1982年   14篇
  1981年   18篇
  1980年   15篇
  1979年   19篇
  1978年   17篇
  1977年   17篇
  1976年   16篇
  1975年   15篇
  1974年   16篇
  1973年   14篇
  1967年   12篇
  1950年   13篇
排序方式: 共有1874条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
921.
922.
923.
924.
925.
This paper describes the 2-year post-treatment follow-up of preschool children identified as having high levels of disruptive behavior at kindergarten entry. They were assigned to four treatment conditions: A no-treatment group, parent-training only, treatment classroom only, and the combination of parent training with the treatment classroom. Interventions lasted the entire kindergarten academic year. Initial post-treatment results reported previously indicated no effects for the parent-training program but some efficacy for the classroom intervention program. For this report, the disruptive behavior (DB) children were subdivided into those who did (n = 74) and did not (n = 77) receive the treatment classroom. Two-year post-treatment follow-up results indicated no differences between the classroom treated and untreated DB groups. These groups also failed to differ in the percentage of children using available treatments across the follow-up period. The DB children in both groups had significantly more symptoms of ADHD and ODD than a community control group (N = 47) at follow-up. They also received higher ratings of externalizing problems on the parent Child Behavior Checklist, more severe ratings of behavior problems at home, and ratings of more pervasive behavior problems at school, and had poorer academic skills. Results suggested that early intervention classrooms for DB children may not produce enduring effects once treatment is withdrawn, and that better approaches are needed for identifying those DB children at greatest risk for later maladjustment.  相似文献   
926.
An experiment by McCann and Besner (1987) suggested that non-words that are homophonic with a high-frequency word (e.g. “keap”) are named in the same amount of time as non-words that are homophonic with a low-frequency word (e.g. “fome”), despite such pseudohomophones being faster to name than non-pseudohomophones (e.g. “feap”, “yome”). Such a result is a challenge to any model of lexical processing that places the word-frequency effect at the stage when the word is being selected from amongst all lexical entries. McCann and Besner propose a complex alternative account where frequency effects arise at a later stage of processing, a stage that is not involved in the naming of pseudohomophones. The purpose of the present study is to look for a frequency effect in the naming of pseudohomophones, controlling for orthographic factors that McCann and Besner ignored. The first experiment, using a homophone decision task, establishes a set of items that produce a clear frequency effect. These items are then used in a naming experiment which also reveals a clear frequency effect for those subjects who make use of a lexical route when pronouncing pseudohomophones. It is concluded from this that the complex model developed by McCann and Besner is unnecessary.  相似文献   
927.
928.
929.
930.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号