全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3352篇 |
免费 | 34篇 |
专业分类
3386篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 35篇 |
2023年 | 39篇 |
2022年 | 31篇 |
2021年 | 58篇 |
2020年 | 70篇 |
2019年 | 109篇 |
2018年 | 126篇 |
2017年 | 131篇 |
2016年 | 146篇 |
2015年 | 109篇 |
2014年 | 101篇 |
2013年 | 360篇 |
2012年 | 196篇 |
2011年 | 191篇 |
2010年 | 111篇 |
2009年 | 92篇 |
2008年 | 140篇 |
2007年 | 137篇 |
2006年 | 96篇 |
2005年 | 132篇 |
2004年 | 118篇 |
2003年 | 112篇 |
2002年 | 82篇 |
2001年 | 56篇 |
2000年 | 56篇 |
1999年 | 39篇 |
1998年 | 29篇 |
1997年 | 19篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 24篇 |
1992年 | 22篇 |
1991年 | 27篇 |
1990年 | 34篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1988年 | 26篇 |
1987年 | 23篇 |
1986年 | 24篇 |
1985年 | 20篇 |
1984年 | 24篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 13篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 7篇 |
1968年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有3386条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
871.
Quarreling couples caught in destructive patterns of communication often cause difficult situations during couple therapy. Such communication patterns range from pure verbal humiliation and insults up to outbursts of violence. In order to remain capable of therapy, psychotherapists must know the underlying mechanisms that provoke and maintain the destructive viscious circles. Moreover, they must have at their disposal the required explanatory ideas and practical intervention strategies for de-escalation and interruption of these destructive patterns. 相似文献
872.
In adults, three phenomena are taken to demonstrate an experience effect on face recognition: an inversion effect, a non-native face effect (so-called 'other-race' effect) and their interaction. It is crucial for our understanding of the developmental perception mechanisms of object processing to discover when these effects are present in childhood. Three- to 5-year-old Caucasian children (N = 64) were asked to recognize upright and inverted Caucasian and Asian faces. Recognition was tested with a forced-choice procedure. Overall performance improved with age. However, there was an interaction between the inversion and non-native effects that did not change with age between ages 3 and 5: (a) the inversion effect with native (Caucasian) faces was larger than with non-native (Asian) faces, and (b) upright native faces were recognized better than upright non-native faces. These results show that face orientation and morphology constrain face processing in 3- to 5-year-olds. The first 3 years of life during which the brain and the environment interact are sufficient to build a face-processing system that constrains recognition. 相似文献
873.
Valence of distractor words increases the effects of irrelevant speech on serial recall 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Participants memorized target words in silence or while ignoring neutral or valent (positive or negative) distractor words that could be either possessor-relevant or other-relevant. Distractor words impaired recall performance, but valent distractor words caused more disruption than neutral distractors, and negative distractors caused more disruption than positive distractors. The results are problematic for explanations of the irrelevant speech effect within working memory models that do not specify an explicit role of attention in the maintenance of information for immediate serial recall. 相似文献
874.
Processing of temporal information: Evidence from eye movements 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
In two experiments, we recorded eye movements to study how readers monitor temporal order information contained in narrative texts. Participants read short texts containing critical temporal information in the sixth sentence, which could be either consistent or inconsistent with temporal order information given in the second sentence. In Experiment 1, inconsistent sentences yielded more regressions to the second sentence and longer refixations of it. In Experiment 2, this pattern of eye movements was shown only by readers who noticed the inconsistency and were able to report it. Theoretical and methodological implications of the results for research on text comprehension are discussed. 相似文献
875.
Rey de Castro A 《The Psychoanalytic quarterly》2003,72(1):131-155
Two case vignettes illustrating different ways of listening to clinical material are presented. The author discusses some limitations of clinical psychoanalytic theory that stem from the fact that primary unconscious processes are, by their very nature, impossible to describe in a language regulated by secondary processes. Hegelian dialectics, first addressed in psychoanalysis by Lacan and later elaborated in the work of Green, as well as the use of paradox by Winnicott and the formalistic approaches of Matte Blanco and Bion, are briefly reviewed as alternative formulas. As psychoanalysts, we are condemned to live with doubt, and neither clinical theories nor metapsychology offer escape from this reality. 相似文献
876.
The central question addressed by this article is whether courses of treatment consisting of pharmacotherapy or pharmacotherapy combined with psychotherapy (combined therapy) produce different changes in personality pathology at follow-up after 40 weeks. We also examined whether recovery from depression has an influence on outcome. The study population consisted of 128 outpatients in whom personality pathology and severity of depression were determined at the start of the study. For 72 patients, personality pathology and severity of depression were determined again after 40 weeks. Of the group of 72 patients, 25 patients received only pharmacotherapy for 6 months, and 47 patients received combined treatment (pharmacotherapy and psychodynamic supportive psychotherapy). The antidepressant protocol provides for three successive steps in case of intolerance or inefficacy: fluoxetine, amitriptyline, and moclobemide. The combined therapy condition consisted of 16 sessions of Short Psychodynamic Supportive Psychotherapy in addition to pharmacotherapy. In the combined therapy condition there was a significant reduction in personality pathology in patients who recovered from depression but also in patients who had not. In the pharmacotherapy condition the significant decrease was restricted to patients who recovered from depression. The results were most striking for Cluster C psychopatology. Patients with cluster B pathology changed the least. Depressed patients with comorbid personality pathology appear to benefit most from a combination of pharmacotherapy and a form of short, psychodynamic, supportive psychotherapy. 相似文献
877.
Salgado JF Anderson N Moscoso S Bertua C de Fruyt F Rolland JP 《The Journal of applied psychology》2003,88(6):1068-1081
A comprehensive meta-analysis of the validity of general mental ability (GMA) measures across 12 occupational categories in the European Community (EC) is presented. GMA measures showed that there is validity generalization and large operational validities for job performance and training success in 11 occupational groups. Results also showed that job complexity moderated the magnitude of the operational validity of GMA tests across three levels of job complexity: low, medium, and high. In general, results were similar to those found in the United States, although the European findings showed a slightly larger magnitude of operational validity in some cases. Theoretical and practical implications of these findings for personnel selection are discussed. ((c) 2003 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
878.
Jan?Peter?de?RuiterEmail author Stéphane?Rossignol Louis?Vuurpijl Douglas?W.?Cunningham Willem?J.?M.?Levelt 《Behavior research methods》2003,35(3):408-419
In this article, we present the spatial logistics task (SLOT) platform for investigating multimodal communication between
2 human participants. Presented are the SLOT communication task and the software and hardware that has been developed to run
SLOT experiments and record the participants’ multimodal behavior. SLOT offers a high level of flexibility in varying the
context of the communication and is particularly useful in studies of the relationship between pen gestures and speech. We
illustrate the use of the SLOT platform by discussing the results of some early experiments. The first is an experiment on
negotiation with a one-way mirror between the participants, and the second is an exploratory study of automatic recognition
of spontaneous pen gestures. The results of these studies demonstrate the usefulness of the SLOT platform for conducting multimodal
communication research in both human-human and human-computer interactions. 相似文献
879.
A.?BergerEmail author K.?-M.?Scheibe S.?Michaelis W.?J.?Streich 《Behavior research methods》2003,35(3):458-466
A biorhythmical method has been developed to assess behavior patterns and to evaluate the living conditions of animals that
can be applied also to humans. All kinds of continuous and equidistant long-term recordings of behavior are suitable for this
method. Time functions from an automatic telemetry system (ETHOSYSII) were analyzed macroscopically (comparison of daily levels)
and microscopically (autocorrelation function and power spectral analysis). Degrees of functional coupling (DFCs) were calculated
to identify and evaluate disturbances in behavior. Hierarchic frequency tuning of complex rhythmic functions of behaviors,
leads primarily to period lengths that are synchronized with the 24-h period. DFCs, a measure of harmony between internal
rhythms and the external 24-h period, were found to be high in well-adapted, healthy, and undisturbed individuals but were
lowered during periods of adaptation, sickness, or social interaction. Specific stress conditions could be identified and
evaluated in several species, under various conditions, using these biorhythmic analyses. 相似文献
880.
Recent studies on perceptual organization in humans claim that the ability to represent a visual scene as a set of coherent
surfaces is of central importance for visual cognition. We examined whether this surface representation hypothesis generalizes
to a non-mammalian species, the barn owl (Tyto alba). Discrimination transfer combined with random-dot stimuli provided the appropriate means for a series of two behavioural
experiments with the specific aims of (1) obtaining psychophysical measurements of figure–ground segmentation in the owl,
and (2) determining the nature of the information involved. In experiment 1, two owls were trained to indicate the presence
or absence of a central planar surface (figure) among a larger region of random dots (ground) based on differences in texture.
Without additional training, the owls could make the same discrimination when figure and ground had reversed luminance, or
were camouflaged by the use of uniformly textured random-dot stereograms. In the latter case, the figure stands out in depth
from the ground when positional differences of the figure in two retinal images are combined (binocular disparity). In experiment 2,
two new owls were trained to distinguish three-dimensional objects from holes using random-dot kinematograms. These birds
could make the same discrimination when information on surface segmentation was unexpectedly switched from relative motion
to half-occlusion. In the latter case, stereograms were used that provide the impression of stratified surfaces to humans
by giving unpairable image features to the eyes. The ability to use image features such as texture, binocular disparity, relative
motion, and half-occlusion interchangeably to determine figure–ground relationships suggests that in owls, as in humans, the
structuring of the visual scene critically depends on how indirect image information (depth order, occlusion contours) is allocated between different surfaces.
Electronic Publication 相似文献