全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1645篇 |
免费 | 118篇 |
专业分类
1763篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 17篇 |
2022年 | 17篇 |
2021年 | 31篇 |
2020年 | 59篇 |
2019年 | 39篇 |
2018年 | 70篇 |
2017年 | 66篇 |
2016年 | 80篇 |
2015年 | 66篇 |
2014年 | 57篇 |
2013年 | 252篇 |
2012年 | 88篇 |
2011年 | 102篇 |
2010年 | 72篇 |
2009年 | 68篇 |
2008年 | 108篇 |
2007年 | 85篇 |
2006年 | 57篇 |
2005年 | 43篇 |
2004年 | 37篇 |
2003年 | 48篇 |
2002年 | 43篇 |
2001年 | 44篇 |
2000年 | 37篇 |
1999年 | 20篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1938年 | 3篇 |
1937年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有1763条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
71.
72.
Moscoso del Prado Martín F Bertram R Häikiö T Schreuder R Baayen RH 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2004,30(6):1271-1278
Finnish has a very productive morphology in which a stem can give rise to several thousand words. This study presents a visual lexical decision experiment addressing the processing consequences of the huge productivity of Finnish morphology. The authors observed that in Finnish words with larger morphological families elicited shorter response latencies. However, in contrast to Dutch and Hebrew, it is not the complete morphological family of a complex Finnish word that codetermines response latencies but only the subset of words directly derived from the complex word itself. Comparisons with parallel experiments using translation equivalents in Dutch and Hebrew showed substantial cross-language predictivity of family size between Finnish and Dutch but not between Finnish and Hebrew, reflecting the different ways in which the Hebrew and Finnish morphological systems contribute to the semantic organization of concepts in the mental lexicon. 相似文献
73.
In line with the ironic processing theory of Wegner (Psychol. Rev. 101 (1994) 34), it is often argued that the suppression of anxiety-related thoughts results in a paradoxical increase of anxiety and thought intrusions, both after and during the thought suppression. In a sample of undergraduate students (14 men, 18 women), we investigated the effects of suppressing anxious thoughts about an imminent painful electrocutaneous stimulus. During thought suppression, self-reported anxiety and frequency of anxious thoughts did not increase, and duration of anxious thoughts decreased. After thought suppression, participants experienced an increase in self-reported anxiety and the frequency of anxious thoughts. There was no effect upon thought duration. The results support the idea that suppression of anxiety-related thoughts may result in a paradoxical increase in anxiety, and may cause and/or maintain anxiety problems. 相似文献
74.
The study set out to examine parents' evaluations of the school they had attended and the role of these evaluations in the formation of parental attitudes towards education. A group of vocationally educated (N = 343) and a group of academically educated (N = 231) parents were asked to think back to their primary school days and evaluate the functioning of their school; they were also requested to indicate their opinions on a set of attitude statements and to predict their preschool-aged child's future direction in secondary education. The parents' evaluation of their schools averaged at satisfactory, with the highest grade given to the quality of the instruction and the lowest grades to the giving of encouragement and the capability of taking individual needs into consideration. The school evaluations were organized by the school generation and, in particular, by the educational position of the subject, so that those parents who had gone to the (new) comprehensive school and those who were university-educated gave more positive evaluations of their schools than did those who had gone to the (old) folk school and those who were vocationally educated. The critical educational attitudes corresponding to the educational position of the academically educated parents were enhanced if their evaluation of their own school was negative, whereas the positive educational attitudes corresponding to the educational position of the vocationally educated parents were enhanced if their evaluation of their own school was positive. With the vocationally educated parents, their school evaluation also organized their prediction of their child's secondary education: a positive evaluation raised and a negative evaluation lowered the likelihood of vocational education as the predicted alternative. 相似文献
75.
Metsäpelto RL Pulkkinen L Poikkeus AM 《Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs》2001,127(2):169-192
Cross-situational stability in parents' emotional warmth and guidance was studied by observing parents (N = 77, M age = 38 years) with their school-aged child in 2 dyadic problem-solving situations and in a family discussion concerning a moral dilemma. The observational data were coded by independent observers using dimensional ratings and dichotomous frequency counts as the 2 coding procedures. These procedures yielded a similar pattern of findings. Parents tended to behave consistently across situations, although the type of situation did affect the amount of emotional warmth and guidance manifested by the parent. Stability was further analyzed by means of structural equation modeling to test whether variance in parents' emotional warmth and guidance across situations was attributable to a generalized parenting style factor. A Parenting Style factor was identified that reflected the parents' child-centeredness; this factor explained, in part, parental behavior within each situation, although contextual factors also contributed to situation-specific variations from task to task. 相似文献
76.
This study examined the organization of self-knowledge, with special attention to beliefs about physical appearance, in three groups of college-aged women: high body dissatisfaction with symptoms of disordered eating; high body dissatisfaction with no symptoms of disorder; and low body dissatisfaction. In the nondisordered, dissatisfied group, negative beliefs about physical appearance were organized in the self-structure in a way that isolated those beliefs and might minimize their impact and importance. This group also displayed adaptive types of self-concept organization (evaluative integration for those with important negative self-beliefs and compartmentalization for those with important positive beliefs) and effective coping strategies. Features of self-structure that characterize the nondisordered,dissatisfied group may provide a useful model for helping individuals with disordered eating cope with their negative physical appearance beliefs. 相似文献
77.
Michael Rönnlund Lars Nyberg Lars Bäckman Lars-Göran Nilsson 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2013,20(3):182-201
Motor activity during encoding of verbal information has been suggested to reduce age differences in episodic memory. Here we examined memory for sentences encoded with enactment (SPTs, subject-performed tasks) or without enactment (VTs, verbal tasks) in a population-based sample consisting of 10 groups ranging in age from 35 to 80 years (N = 1000). Memory performance was assessed by immediate free- and category-cued recall. Degree of clustering was measured by the adjusted ratio of clustering score. Recall of cognitive activities served as a complementary measure of memory for performed tasks. Sentence recall showed a gradual decline across age, of about the same magnitude for SPTs and VTs, in both free and cued recall. Clustering in free recall was higher for SPTs than for VTs, but there were no age differences in clustering. A pattern of gradual decline from age 35 was observed also in activity recall, regardless of whether the activities involved motor activity or not. Across the memory measures, differences in education accounted for all of the age-related variance in performance among the younger (35-55 years) but not the older groups (60-80 years), suggesting that genuine aging effects in these measures are more prominent in old age. Together, the results indicate that age differences in episodic memory, in line with most, if not all, types of encoding support, generalize across the performed/non-performed distinction. 相似文献
78.
Jürgen Rehm Waldemar Lilli Birgit van Eimeren 《European journal of social psychology》1988,18(4):375-379
A quasi-experiment was conducted to examine the effects of self-categorization in overlapping categories on intergroup differentiation. ‘Old aged women’ was used as the first, ‘sports’ as the additional category. It could be shown that intergroup differentiation was significantly reduced under crossed categorization conditions. Implications for social compensation strategies are discussed. 相似文献
79.
In an attempt to study remote memory for visuospatial information, geographical knowledge was examined in patient J.N., who had received a radical right temporal lobectomy to remove a large glioblastoma. In postoperative tests J.N. displayed severe deficits in anterograde memory for visuospatial and verbal material, visuoperceptual functions, constructions, attention, and response initiation. Despite her global cognitive deficiencies she performed normally on the WAIS Information Scale and the Fargo Map Test, a measure of geographical knowledge. In contrast, patients known to have severe amnesia exhibited impairments on tests of geographical knowledge that required precise localization of features such as cities. Assuming that J.N.'s performance is typical of patients with extensive damage to the right temporal lobe, the findings suggest that the integrity of the right temporal lobe is not essential for the retrieval of premorbidly established visuospatial memories. 相似文献
80.
Torbjörn Tännsjö 《Philosophical Studies》2007,135(2):123-143
Moral relativism comes in many varieties. One is a moral doctrine, according to which we ought to respect other cultures,
and allow them to solve moral problems as they see fit. I will say nothing about this kind of moral relativism in the present
context. Another kind of moral relativism is semantic moral relativism, according to which, when we pass moral judgements,
we make an implicit reference to some system of morality (our own). According to this kind of moral relativism, when I say
that a certain action is right, my statement is elliptic. What I am really saying is that, according to the system of morality
in my culture, this action is right. I will reject this kind of relativism. According to yet another kind of moral relativism,
which we may call epistemic, it is possible that, when one person (belonging to one culture) makes a certain moral judgement,
such as that this action is right, and another person (belong to another culture) makes the judgement that the very same action
is wrong, they may have just as good reasons for their respective judgements; it is even possible that, were they fully informed
about all the facts, equally imaginative, and so forth, they would still hold on to their respective (conflicting) judgements.
They are each fully justified in their belief in conflicting judgements. I will comment on this form of moral relativism in
passing. Finally, however, there is a kind of moral relativism we could call ontological, according to which, when two persons
pass conflicting moral verdicts on a certain action, they may both be right. The explanation is that they make their judgements
from the perspective of different, socially constructed, moral universes. So while it is true in the first person's moral
universe that a certain action is right, it is true in the second person's moral universe that the very same action is wrong.
I explain and defend this version of ontological moral relativism. 相似文献