全文获取类型
收费全文 | 250篇 |
免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
255篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 5篇 |
2017年 | 3篇 |
2016年 | 8篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 6篇 |
2013年 | 17篇 |
2012年 | 3篇 |
2011年 | 4篇 |
2010年 | 10篇 |
2009年 | 4篇 |
2008年 | 3篇 |
2007年 | 7篇 |
2006年 | 6篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 5篇 |
2003年 | 5篇 |
2002年 | 9篇 |
2001年 | 12篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 4篇 |
1969年 | 4篇 |
1964年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有255条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
161.
Bill Drysdale 《Journal of constructivist psychology》2013,26(3):271-286
Hypotheses derived from the literature on pain and on personal construct theory were tested using the repertory grid technique on samples of acute and chronic low back pain patients. Two main differences emerged between these groups: (a) Chronic pain patients showed a small but significant tendency to associate being in pain with being sensitive to others; and (b) chronic pain patients perceived significantly less anger around them. The two groups were then pooled to examine changes in construing with increasing pain duration. Two main trends were noted with increasing chronicity: (a) Pain patients perceived significantly less depression around them, with the construct depressed/not depressed becoming increasingly subordinate, and (b) pain was seen in increasingly less negative terms. The results are discussed with reference to the literature and suggestions are made for future research areas. 相似文献
162.
Bill Hastings Julia Kiely Trevor Watkins 《Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management》2013,33(2):43-51
This study examines the use of travel incentives as motivators for a commission only life insurance sales force in the United Kingdom. A sample of 1,083 sales representatives reported that travel is a strong motivator and valued more highly than several other types of incentives. 相似文献
163.
This article focuses on the kinds of evaluative judgments made when applying DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria within the diagnostic interview between clinician and patient. The authors name these kinds of value judgments in diagnosis "normative warrant" because they involve one or more justifications (warrants) for standard-bearing (normative) elements involved in applying diagnostic criteria to actual patients. Seven types of normative warrant judgments are described (Type 1, Semantic-Phenomenal Matching; Type 2, Solicitation Choice; Type 3, Sociocultural Context; Type 4, Performance-Context Matching; Type 5, Deviance Threshold; Type 6, Threshold Characterization; Type 7, Disvalue characterization) and the typology is illustrated by applying it to various DSM-IV-TR personality disorder criteria. A research and clinical understanding of normative warrant may well contribute to the refinement of criteria sets as well as the refinement of the clinical use of criteria sets. 相似文献
164.
165.
This review article argues for a better acknowledgement by penal philosophers of the diversity of subjects, agents, and practices of punishment. Much current penal philosophy has an unhelpful hyper-focus on the criminal punishment of culpable adults, by states, often through imprisonment. This paradigmatic case is important, but other subjects, agents, and practices of punishment are not statistically insignificant side-issues, and a comprehensive account of punishment should address them. Our understanding of punishment as a whole can be enhanced by considering non-paradigmatic punishment, with implications for whether and when punishment is justified, how we should understand appropriate authority, and how we should understand and engage with abolitionist arguments. We explore non-paradigmatic penal practices (community punishments, suspended prison sentence, restorative justice, and alternative jurisprudence), non-paradigmatic punishing agents (International judicial bodies, schools, and religious communities; with practices such as boycotts, shaming and shunning) and non-paradigmatic subjects of punishment (collective agents, corporations and children). 相似文献
166.
Natural languages make prolific use of conventional constituent‐ordering patterns to indicate “who did what to whom,” yet the mechanisms through which these regularities arise are not well understood. A series of recent experiments demonstrates that, when prompted to express meanings through silent gesture, people bypass native language conventions, revealing apparent biases underpinning word order usage, based on the semantic properties of the information to be conveyed. We extend the scope of these studies by focusing, experimentally and computationally, on the interpretation of silent gesture. We show cross‐linguistic experimental evidence that people use variability in constituent order as a cue to obtain different interpretations. To illuminate the computational principles that govern interpretation of non‐conventional communication, we derive a Bayesian model of interpretation via biased inductive inference and estimate these biases from the experimental data. Our analyses suggest people's interpretations balance the ambiguity that is characteristic of emerging language systems, with ordering preferences that are skewed and asymmetric, but defeasible. 相似文献
167.
Bill E. Owenby 《Journal of Creativity in Mental Health》2017,12(1):146-159
Chandler identified eight generalized benefits of animal-assisted therapy (AAT) for a person’s growth. These beneficial areas include (a) motivation, (b) distress tolerance, (c) alternative form of nurturance, (d) physical soothing, (e) genuine acceptance, (f) interactional enjoyment, (g) increased trust, and (h) increased encouragement to overcome barriers. Homestead identified supervision ideals, skills, and barriers within the supervisory alliance. If supervision is negative, this experience may disrupt the supervision relationship, process, and requirements. If not properly addressed, potential harm may come to the supervisee, supervisor, and possibly the client’s welfare. This review will identify how these eight areas of AAT may benefit the supervisory alliance to ensure the best care for the supervisee, supervisor, and the client. 相似文献
168.
Klaus G. Melchers Benedikt Bill Anne-Kathrin Buehl Katrin Rybczynski Jana Kühnel 《European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology》2020,29(5):728-739
ABSTRACT Previous research on interviewees’ ability to identify criteria (ATIC) found that interview performance is positively related to the degree to which interviewees identify the targeted performance criteria in a selection interview. Similarly, the use of self-promotion as an impression management tactic is also positively related to interview performance. The present study extended this research and used multilevel analyses to investigate whether intraindividual variation concerning identification of the performance criteria and the use of self-promotion is related to within-person performance fluctuations across interview questions. Furthermore, we also tested whether correct identification of the performance dimensions moderates the effects of self-promotion behaviour on interview performance. In a simulated selection interview, we found that ATIC and self-promotion were indeed positively related to interview performance on the between-person level (i.e., for the entire interview). However, no moderator effect was found. Furthermore, on the within-person level interviewees’ performance was better concerning questions for which they were better at identifying the performance criteria but worse concerning questions for which they used more self-promotion. Thus, identification of the targeted dimensions and self-promotion have diverging effects on the level of specific interview questions. Finally, we discuss implications for research and practice. 相似文献
169.
The debate on the use or nonuse of policy evaluation frequently starts from the normative assumption that is a rational and
rewarding activity for governments to pursue. Failure to use evaluation is assumed to indicate political and organizational
inefficiencies and weaknesses that can, and should, be corrected. This article argues that this is too simplistic an assumption
that abstracts evaluation from the political and social context in which it is developed and utilized. In particular, the
way that governments develop and use evaluation is conditioned, first by the macroeconomic climate in which they operate and
second, by the ways they choose to respond to this and by the microeconomics of the budgetary process. These issues are explored
in the context of recent developments in British central government. here a tight fiscal policy and a concern with administrative
costs have led to move from top-down rational systems of evaluation to more pragmatic, bottom-up concerns with performance
measures and performance indicators. It is concluded that policy evaluation is unlikely to regain and enhance its credibility
unless a reshaping of administrative structures and processes take place.
Andrew Gray and Bill Jenkins are respectively senior lecturer in adminiistrative studies and senior lecturer in interdisciplinary
studies at the University of Kent at Canterbury, England. They have worked for a number of years on the development of management
reforms in the UK government and have recently been involved in a research project on the evolution of the Financial Management
Initiative (FMI) in British central government departments. Their publications includeAdministrative Politics in British Govenment (Wheatsheaf Books, 1985) and articles in such journals asFinancial Accountability and Management, Public Administration, andPublic Money and Management. 相似文献
170.
This study evaluated the intensity of delusional beliefs during the modification of delusional verbalizations with three chronically mentally ill inpatients. The multiple-baseline across responses design was used to evaluate the efficacy of a behavioral treatment package to modify delusional verbalizations. Results revealed that changes were obtained in delusional verbalizations to personal questions without corresponding changes in the subjects' delusional beliefs. Changes in delusional verbalizations were well-maintained for two subjects during a 6-month follow-up period. Mixed results were obtained for generalization probe interviews. 相似文献