首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4061篇
  免费   176篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2020年   55篇
  2019年   87篇
  2018年   109篇
  2017年   97篇
  2016年   103篇
  2015年   83篇
  2014年   108篇
  2013年   446篇
  2012年   131篇
  2011年   158篇
  2010年   98篇
  2009年   100篇
  2008年   149篇
  2007年   131篇
  2006年   130篇
  2005年   111篇
  2004年   98篇
  2003年   106篇
  2002年   124篇
  2001年   108篇
  2000年   103篇
  1999年   75篇
  1998年   49篇
  1997年   41篇
  1996年   45篇
  1995年   46篇
  1994年   45篇
  1993年   43篇
  1992年   57篇
  1991年   56篇
  1990年   55篇
  1989年   56篇
  1988年   40篇
  1987年   40篇
  1986年   52篇
  1985年   55篇
  1984年   59篇
  1983年   42篇
  1982年   48篇
  1981年   44篇
  1979年   48篇
  1978年   36篇
  1976年   36篇
  1975年   40篇
  1974年   41篇
  1973年   38篇
  1972年   35篇
  1970年   41篇
  1968年   37篇
  1967年   44篇
排序方式: 共有4239条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
861.
862.
863.
864.
The development of a secondary reinforcer as a result of associating a neutral stimulus (buzzer) with intravenous (IV) doses of morphine was studied in rats. Secondary reinforcement developed in the absence of physical dependence and followed the association of the stimulus with either response-contingent or non-contingent injections of morphine. Strength of the conditioned reinforcer, measured in terms of responding on a lever for the stimulus plus infusion of saline solution, was proportional to the unit dosage of morphine employed in pairings of buzzer and drug. When extinction of the lever-press response for IV morphine was conducted (by substituting saline for morphine solution) in the absence of the conditioned reinforcing stimulus, it was seen later that the stimulus could still elicit lever responses, until it too had been present for a sufficient interval of non-reinforced responding. Similarly, extinction of the response for morphine by blocking its action with naloxone in the absence of the stimulus did not eliminate the conditioned reinforcement. Another study showed that a passive, subcutaneous (SC) dose of morphine served to maintain lever-pressing on a contingency of buzzer plus sahne infusion. Furthermore, the stimuli resulting from the presence of morphine (after a SC injection) were able to reinstate the lever-responding with only the buzzer-saline contingency when such responses had previously been extinguished. Moreover, it was shown thatd-amphetamine could restore responding under the same conditions, and that morphine could also do so for rats in which the primary reinforcer had beend-amphetamine. It is suggested that animal data such as these show that procedures designed for the elimination of human drug-taking behavior must take into account secondary reinforcers as well as the primary reinforcer(s).  相似文献   
865.
Past studies have shown that disruptive behavior can be eliminated and attending behavior accelerated in an academic setting. The relationship between these behaviors and academic performance is not well understood. The effects of behavioral and performance contingencies on classroom behavior and on academic performance were investigated. The subjects, third-grade students from an inner city elementary school, were exposed to a series of conditions including baseline, behavior contingencies, performance contingencies, and a mix of behavior and performance contingencies using a reversal design. The students worked 100 randomly selected mathematics problems for 20 minutes each day during each period. Behavioral contingencies improved attending and decreased disruptions but did not improve performance. Performance contingencies increased per cent correct problems but attending declined and disruptions increased. The combined contingencies increased both performance and attending. The experiment was replicated with another class of children varying the sequence of conditions and the amount of token reinforcement that could be earned. The findings emphasized the importance of designing specific contingencies for specific target behaviors. Behavioral contingencies did not have the positive effect on performance often implied, nor were performance contingencies alone able to maintain acceptable classroom behavior.  相似文献   
866.
867.
868.
Adaptation to spectacles that alter in equivalent amounts the accommodation and the convergence with which objects are viewed was produced under two conditions. In one, S alternately pushed away or pulled toward him a screen that exhibited only a single vertical contour while wearing glaaaes that caused decreases in accommodation and convergence equivalent to 1.5 lens diopters. Here kinesthesis for these arm movements provided the only veridical distance cues, A small, but highly significant, adaptation effect was obtained with a teat in which S, before and after the adaptation period, pointed to the location of a test line in the distance dimension. Corresponding tests consisting in size and in depth estimates did not show an adaptation effect. In the other condition of adaptation, S moved objects by hand toward and away from himself while wearing spectacles that increased accommodation and convergence by the equivalent of 1.5 lens diopters. In addition to the altered oculomotor cues, some veridical visual cues for distance such as are caused by perspective were present. This condition yielded changes in size and depth estimates indicative of an adaptation in visual distance perception and a larger effect of adaptation measured by the pointing test. We concluded that the excess of the adaptation effect measured by pointing over that measured by size estimation represents an adaptation in proprioception, as did the pointing effect produced by our first adaptation condition.  相似文献   
869.
In five experiments, Sa were presented with a variety of sour and bitter compounds after the tongue was rinsed with distilled H20, QHCl, urea, or citric acid. All the acids tested were significantly less sour following adaptation to citric acid than after adaptation to distilled H2O. The taste of these acids was not affected by rinsing the tongue with QHCl or urea. QHCl adaptation markedly reduced the bitterness of some compounds, while having little effect on others, including urea and citric acid. Both urea and citric acid had smaller but reliable effects on the bitterness of QHCI. These apparently incompatible results do not seem to be the result of a simple verbal confusion between sourness and bitterness. Some compounds were not affected by any of the adapting conditions. The coding mechanisms for the sourness of acids appears to be relatively simple, while that for bitterness is more complex.  相似文献   
870.
Rats were trained in a six-choice jumping stand apparatus to enter the dark door, and avoid the five bright doors. Rats with bilateral superior collicular lesions were found to be severely impaired in this task, although further experiments showed that they were able to discriminate bright from dark stimuli and could perform correctly if allowed to approach each door in turn. It is suggested that the superior colliculus is important in orientation to visual cues, and there is some evidence that it is involved in orientation to brightness but not visual form cues.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号