全文获取类型
收费全文 | 552篇 |
免费 | 34篇 |
专业分类
586篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 11篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 17篇 |
2017年 | 15篇 |
2016年 | 24篇 |
2015年 | 14篇 |
2014年 | 19篇 |
2013年 | 64篇 |
2012年 | 24篇 |
2011年 | 30篇 |
2010年 | 19篇 |
2009年 | 21篇 |
2008年 | 24篇 |
2007年 | 25篇 |
2006年 | 33篇 |
2005年 | 19篇 |
2004年 | 22篇 |
2003年 | 15篇 |
2002年 | 18篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1963年 | 2篇 |
1962年 | 2篇 |
1961年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有586条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
22.
Leading to Stimulate Employees' Ideas: A Quantitative Review of Leader–Member Exchange,Employee Voice,Creativity, and Innovative Behavior 下载免费PDF全文
Joel B. Carnevale Lei Huang Marcus Crede Peter Harms Mary Uhl‐Bien 《Psychologie appliquee》2017,66(4):517-552
Through social exchange, leaders can offer relational support or resources to facilitate employees' proactive attempts to bring positive change (voice) or novel ideas (creativity) and behaviors (innovative behavior) to their work. We consider these three outcomes under the same nomological network as they all represent employees' idea contribution to the organisation. The present paper thus meta‐analytically reviews the findings of research relating leader–member exchange (LMX) to voice (37 samples), creativity (53 samples), and innovative behavior (29 samples). Results show that LMX positively predicts voice, creativity, and innovative behavior. Moreover, LMX is more strongly related with creativity than with voice or innovative behavior, a significant difference maintained even after controlling for study characteristics that may act as confounding variables. Implications of our findings and directions for future research are also discussed. 相似文献
23.
Michael N. Dalili Lawrence Schofield-Toloza Marcus R. Munafò Ian S. Penton-Voak 《Cognition & emotion》2017,31(5):858-867
Many cognitive bias modification (CBM) tasks use facial expressions of emotion as stimuli. Some tasks use unique facial stimuli, while others use composite stimuli, given evidence that emotion is encoded prototypically. However, CBM using composite stimuli may be identity- or emotion-specific, and may not generalise to other stimuli. We investigated the generalisability of effects using composite faces in two experiments. Healthy adults in each study were randomised to one of four training conditions: two stimulus-congruent conditions, where same faces were used during all phases of the task, and two stimulus-incongruent conditions, where faces of the opposite sex (Experiment 1) or faces depicting another emotion (Experiment 2) were used after the modification phase. Our results suggested that training effects generalised across identities. However, our results indicated only partial generalisation across emotions. These findings suggest effects obtained using composite stimuli may extend beyond the stimuli used in the task but remain emotion-specific. 相似文献
24.
25.
The authors investigated the factor structure and construct validity of the Fraboni Scale of Ageism and the age and gender differences in ageism scores. Confirmatory factor analyses supported the multidimensional nature of FSA scores and generally corroborated the initial factor structure reported by M. Fraboni, with some notable exceptions. Essentially, the present findings were aligned with theoretical models of ageism that emphasize both cognitive facets and affective facets. That is, on the basis of their factor analytic findings, the authors redefined Fraboni's original factors of Antilocution, Avoidance, and Discrimination as Stereotypes, Separation, and Affective Attitudes, respectively, because of the clustering of items within factors. The revised 3-factor structure accounted for 36.4% of the variance in FSA scores. FSA factor scores significantly related to other scores from other measures of age-related attitudes, with higher correlations among factors that were similar in terms of their cognitive nature versus their affective nature. Finally, younger individuals and men had significantly higher ageism scores on the FSA than older individuals and women. The authors discussed the importance of adequately assessing ageism, with particular emphasis devoted to the understanding of age bias. 相似文献
26.
27.
This study prospectively examined body image in a community sample of women smokers (N = 141, 89% European–American, M age = 39.5, M BMI = 25.5) participating in a 12-week randomized smoking cessation trial. Participants completed measures assessing subjective and perceptual aspects of body image. Regression analyses showed that subjective body image was predictive of smoking rate at the end of treatment. A MANCOVA revealed that perceptual body image (body size estimation) significantly predicted smoking cessation. Quitting smoking actually worsened body image, although these results were no longer significant after controlling for weight gain. Results support the theory that a negative body image may hamper cessation attempts. In addition, weight gain during smoking cessation may adversely impact body image and lead to an increased risk for relapse. 相似文献
28.
Taking a social psychological approach to metacognitive judgments, this study analyzed the difference in realism (validity) in confidence and frequency judgments (i.e., estimates of overall accuracy) between one's own and another person's answers to general knowledge questions. Experiment 1 showed that when judging their own answers, compared with another's answers, the participants exhibited higher overconfidence, better ability to discriminate correct from incorrect answers, lower accuracy, and lower confidence. However, the overconfidence effect could be attributable to the lowest level of confidence. Furthermore, when heeding additional information about another's answers the participants showed higher confidence and better discrimination ability. The overconfidence effect of Experiment 1 was not found in Experiment 2. However, the results of Experiment 2 were consistent with Experiment 1 in terms of discrimination ability, confidence, and accuracy. Finally, in both experiments the participants gave lower frequency judgments of their own overall accuracy compared with their frequency judgments of another person's overall accuracy. 相似文献
29.
Marcus RF 《Adolescence》1999,34(133):33-46
One hundred sixty-three young adolescents (72 males, 91 females) were administered a 23-item self-report inventory measuring antisocial behaviors. Results were analyzed separately for males and females. Exploratory factor analysis showed antisocial behavior to be multidimensional and to have different factor structures for males and females. The three factors accounting for more than two-thirds of the variance in antisocial behavior are discussed in terms of specialization in drug/alcohol use and violence. 相似文献
30.
Marcus Kracht 《Journal of Philosophical Logic》2011,40(3):397-420
The transition from form to meaning is not neatly layered: there is no point where form ends and content sets in. Rather,
there is an almost continuous process that converts form into meaning. That process cannot always take a straight line. Very
often we hit barriers in our mind, due to the inability to understand the exact content of the sentence just heard. The standard
division between formula and interpretation (or value) should therefore be given up when talking about the process of understanding.
Interestingly, when we do this it turns out that there are ‘easy’ formulae, those we can understand without further help, and ‘difficult’ ones, which we cannot. 相似文献