首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1204篇
  免费   54篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2023年   11篇
  2021年   18篇
  2020年   25篇
  2019年   22篇
  2018年   22篇
  2017年   20篇
  2016年   27篇
  2015年   21篇
  2014年   19篇
  2013年   142篇
  2012年   47篇
  2011年   43篇
  2010年   26篇
  2009年   24篇
  2008年   37篇
  2007年   46篇
  2006年   40篇
  2005年   39篇
  2004年   28篇
  2003年   41篇
  2002年   33篇
  2001年   18篇
  2000年   25篇
  1999年   21篇
  1998年   13篇
  1997年   14篇
  1996年   20篇
  1995年   12篇
  1994年   11篇
  1993年   12篇
  1992年   13篇
  1991年   17篇
  1990年   9篇
  1989年   17篇
  1988年   15篇
  1987年   15篇
  1986年   19篇
  1985年   22篇
  1984年   13篇
  1983年   14篇
  1982年   22篇
  1981年   16篇
  1980年   11篇
  1979年   10篇
  1978年   14篇
  1976年   19篇
  1975年   13篇
  1973年   13篇
  1972年   11篇
  1971年   12篇
排序方式: 共有1259条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
961.
P M Rosen  B Walsh  P Lucas 《Adolescence》1988,23(89):29-34
This study examined the effects of integrating an unstable group of seriously disturbed adolescents into an existing day treatment program. Behavioral data were collected for an established group of subjects who were in treatment one year prior to and one year after the arrival of a new group of severely disturbed adolescents. It was found that the disruptive behavior displayed by the new admission subjects was not imitated by the subjects of the established group. Rather, there was a significant decrease in aggression and in the overall number of psychiatric events for the established group. The treatment milieu was seen to be resistant to the effects of the new admissions largely due to the structured form of treatment.  相似文献   
962.
In this study of the reliability and validity of the Children's Depression Adjective Check Lists (C-DACL; Brewer & Lubin, 1987) with emotionally disturbed adolescent boys (N=50),data were collected on two occasions. Internal consistency (alpha) was .89 and .94 for form H and .89 and .95 for form I. Alternate form reliabilities were .86 and .95. Split-half reliabilities were .80 and .89 for form H and .68 and .86 for form I. Concurrent validity was determined by correlations between C-DACL and the Self-Rating Scale of Depressed Mood. Correlations were .65 and .80 for H and .54 and .80 for I. Data from the present study were tested against data from emotionally disturbed adolescent girls (Sokoloff & Lubin, 1983), with boys scoring significantly lower on both forms (H and I) of the C-DACL.  相似文献   
963.
Further explorations of common-sense representations of common illnesses   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
A simple method is presented for measuring people's illness cognitions--their common-sense representations of common illnesses. Data were collected from 1,628 different respondents who described a recent illness form 1 to 3 separate times over a 17-month period. A free-clustering task performed by a set of naive participants confirmed that these cognitions fall into the five components that have been previously noted: identity, time line, consequences, cause, and cure. These five components are found to be reasonably stable over time and across different illness episodes. Several consequences of these illness cognitions, in terms of changes in health-locus-of-control beliefs and different propensities to visit a doctor, are also noted. Specifically, controllable attributions for getting sick and personal responsibility attributions for getting better are associated with increased beliefs in Self-Control Over Health and decreased beliefs in Chance Health Outcomes; people with strong Identity and Cure components in their common-sense representations of common illnesses have a greater propensity to visit a doctor when feeling ill.  相似文献   
964.
Ten patients who terminated prematurely from 10 different therapy groups were interviewed using a semistructured interview protocol. Half the subjects were patients in private therapy groups; the other half were patients in clinic therapy groups. Among the issues inquired about were subjects' initial attitudes about group treatment; the nature of the preparation process; subjects' experience of the group therapist, other group members, and the group process; their reasons for leaving their groups; and the nature of the termination process. The findings reported focus on patients' perceptions of therapists' motives; the importance of establishing appropriate treatment goals; the handling of negative feelings about the group and/or the group therapist; the issue of narcissistic injury; the experience of the balance between giving and receiving; the difficulties caused by unresolved conflicts within groups; and the problems posed by inadequate preparation for the group experience. Guidelines for practices aimed at minimizing premature terminations are offered.  相似文献   
965.
966.
In 2 experiments, access to a 0.15% saccharin solution was followed on alternating days by access to a 32% sucrose solution and the same saccharin solution. In Experiment 1, rats increased both intake of and preference for a flavored saccharin solution that predicted sucrose, but neither effect was found using a predictive odor cue alone. Experiment 2 replicated the predictive flavor results but showed suppression of saccharin intake when environmental cues predicted sucrose. When both flavor and environment predicted sucrose, saccharin intake did not change, but preference for the predictive flavor increased. Discriminative taste cues appear to facilitate the development of preference conditioning, but environmental cues favor negative anticipatory contrast effects. Also, preference conditioning and contrast may develop concurrently and compete for expression.  相似文献   
967.
We describe an apparatus for testing laterality in haptic processing in the discrimination of nonsense shapes in humans or monkeys. The system, which permits either mono- or dichhaptic discrimination, automatically provides data on the measurement of accuracy along with information on hand exploratory strategies.  相似文献   
968.
The answer to the question of what health care services should be covered by a managed care plan is straightforward; the plan should cover whatever the consumer is willing to pay for. From the plan's perspective, the consumer is the payer, that is, the employer who negotiates the plan; not the individual patient whose personal preferences and interests may be quite different. Since managed care organizations contract with payers to arrange for health care services within a defined set of benefits, there is a broader question as well: Within the benefits chosen by the payer, what actually is covered? Criteria for determining "medical necessity," which managed care plans frequently use as the basis for coverage, are discussed.  相似文献   
969.
Hong Kong, with a population of over six million in a modern city, has been shown to be a very stressful place to live in. However, epidemiological data from a big-scale study in this regard are lacking. The present territory-wide survey, from a sample representative of the general population, was aimed to investigate the work and family stress of Chinese adults in the community with respect to the impact on their psychological well-being. The psychosocial context was considered as an important source of stress. The results showed that coping with work demands and rearing children were reported as the highest stress responses. Moreover, workers in Hong Kong took an instrumental attitude, i.e., one emphasizing material rewards, in order to reduce the job stress arising from interpersonal conflicts in the workplace.  相似文献   
970.
Six elementary-aged children were taught to spell words containing initial consonant clusters (CCs). They were trained to select printed words in response to the corresponding spoken words using computerized matching-to-sample procedures. After each training session, they were tested for spelling with a constructed-response transfer test. Based on previous selective stimulus control research, we hypothesized that only the first letter of an initial CC might control spelling when CC spelling errors are made. Thus, a critical-difference matching-to-sample training condition that required the children to respond to both letters of the CC to be correct was compared to a multiple-difference training condition that required the children to respond to only one letter of the pair. Results showed that children made fewer errors during the multiple-difference training condition than during the critical-difference training condition. On the constructed-response transfer tests, however, more overall errors and CC errors were made in the multiple-difference condition than in the critical-difference condition, and the words trained in the multiple-difference condition required more training sessions to reach criterion. All children improved their spelling of novel CC words by the completion of training. If normal classroom or home reading was to be supplemented by computer tasks of the kind used here, some spelling problems could be circumvented without costly intervention by a teacher or a special trainer.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号