全文获取类型
收费全文 | 933篇 |
免费 | 17篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
951篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 13篇 |
2018年 | 11篇 |
2017年 | 10篇 |
2016年 | 19篇 |
2015年 | 14篇 |
2014年 | 10篇 |
2013年 | 116篇 |
2012年 | 24篇 |
2011年 | 28篇 |
2010年 | 18篇 |
2009年 | 16篇 |
2008年 | 23篇 |
2007年 | 28篇 |
2006年 | 31篇 |
2005年 | 34篇 |
2004年 | 19篇 |
2003年 | 30篇 |
2002年 | 27篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 16篇 |
1998年 | 11篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 20篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 11篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 12篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 18篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1982年 | 22篇 |
1981年 | 15篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 18篇 |
1975年 | 13篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 12篇 |
1972年 | 7篇 |
1971年 | 11篇 |
1970年 | 7篇 |
1966年 | 7篇 |
1965年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有951条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
291.
The Leaderless Group Discussion Technique 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
292.
It has been shown previously that spoonerisms (such as barn door → darn bore) can be elicited by having subjects attempt to articulate a target barn door) preceded by bias items which contain at least the initial phoneme (/d/) of the desired error outcome. Since certain linguistic characteristics of the error outcomes differ from those of their targets, variables which affect only these ‘outcome’ properties in a systematic way can be shown to be the result of prearticulatory output processes, independent of perceptual ‘target’ properties. The present study shows that the base-rate of errors produced by the phonetic bias technique can be increased dramatically by adding, to the word-pairs preceding the target, some items which are semantically synonymous to the error outcomes of the target. In this way, it is demonstrated rigorously that semantic bias increases the likelihood of slips of the tongue; which is one of the defining properties of so-called ‘Freudian slips’. Implications are discussed. 相似文献
293.
Visual pattern preferences were established for 9- and 13-week-old infants (N = 96) using stimuli varying in contour density presented either at a low, moderate, or high luminance level. Age differences in the maximally preferred patterns across stimuli and luminance levels indicated that luminance interacts with contour density in determining stimulus preference functions for a given age. A neural activation model based on synchronous neural activity was advanced to describe these results. 相似文献
294.
Bernard H. Salzberg B.L. Hopkins Andrew J. Wheeler Linda Taylor 《Journal of School Psychology》1974,12(1):24-30
After measuring the amount of disruptive behavior exhibited by nine kindergarten children in a lunchroom, delayed feedback about the disruptive behaviors engaged in was given to each child after he left the lunchroom, but there were no additional differential consequences for any behaviors. This delayed feedback alone had little effect on the rates of disruptive behavior. When the delayed feedback was paired with contingent access to play, the rates of disruptive behaviors of six of the children was substantially reduced. A more elaborate form of feedback, still paired with contingent access to play, was later found to be effective in reducing the rates of disruptive behavior of the other three children. When contingencies and feedback were removed, the rates of disruptive behavior of the three children who required the extended feedback increased, but the rates of disruptive behavior for the other six children did not increase upon termination of contingencies and feedback. However, when disruptive behavior was differentially reinforced, the rates of disruptive behavior of all of the children increased. 相似文献
295.
296.
297.
298.
299.
Intracerebral muscimol injection is widely used to inactivate discrete brain structures during behavioral tasks. However, little effort has been made to quantify the extent of muscimol diffusion. The authors report here electrophysiological and autoradiographic results obtained after muscimol injection (1 microg/microl) either into the nucleus basalis magnocellularis (0.1-0.4 microl) or into the thalamic reticular nucleus (RE, 0.05-0.1 microl). In 52 rats, multiunit recordings were collected either in the RE or in the auditory thalamus during the 2 h following muscimol injection. Decreases in neuronal activity were observed up to 3 mm from the injection site; their time of occurrence was a function of the distance between the injection and recording sites. Because these decreases cannot be explained by physiological effects, they likely reflected muscimol diffusion up to the recording sites. Autoradiographic studies involved 25 rats and different experimental conditions. Optical density (OD) measures indicated that after a survival time of 15 min, a 0.05 microl injection produced a labeled area of 5.25 mm(2) at the injection site and a rostrocaudal labeling of 1.7 mm. Increasing the survival time to 60 min, or increasing the injected volume to 0.1 microl, systematically led to a larger labeled area at the injection site (8-12 mm(2)) and to a larger rostrocaudal diffusion (2.0-2.5 mm). Direct quantifications of radioactivity by a high-resolution radioimager validated the OD measures and even indicated a larger muscimol diffusion (up to 3.25 mm). Thus, these data point out that muscimol diffusion after intracerebral microinjection is larger than usually supposed. The relationships between these results and those obtained in behavioral studies are discussed. 相似文献
300.
Couples (22 young, married, 18 young unmarried) completed the Exchange Orientation Scale, which measures how much individuals believe equality of exchange should characterize their social relationships, as well as the Norman Personality Trait Scale, which gives measures for the self and ideal-self. Couples also rated their sexual satisfaction, their partners' physical attractiveness, their own attractiveness, and had their photographs evaluated for attractiveness by disinterested raters. Subgroups were formed of physically attractive couples, couples in which the members were of disparate attractiveness, and couples in which the individuals perceived themselves as equal to their partners in attractiveness or as inferior. In accordance with exchange theory, the hypotheses were (1) unmarried men would show higher exchange scores than married men, (2) attractive men in disparately attractive couples would show greater exchange scores than attractive men in both-attractive couples, (3) attractive members of disparately attractive couples would possess lower self-acceptance and (4) experience greater sexual satisfaction than attractive members of both-attractive couples. Results supported all hypotheses in varying extents. 相似文献