全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1660篇 |
免费 | 21篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1682篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 24篇 |
2019年 | 17篇 |
2018年 | 20篇 |
2017年 | 30篇 |
2016年 | 34篇 |
2015年 | 32篇 |
2014年 | 29篇 |
2013年 | 182篇 |
2012年 | 44篇 |
2011年 | 50篇 |
2010年 | 25篇 |
2009年 | 25篇 |
2008年 | 45篇 |
2007年 | 51篇 |
2006年 | 45篇 |
2005年 | 51篇 |
2004年 | 35篇 |
2003年 | 49篇 |
2002年 | 46篇 |
2001年 | 30篇 |
2000年 | 27篇 |
1999年 | 28篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 24篇 |
1996年 | 29篇 |
1995年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 22篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 31篇 |
1991年 | 25篇 |
1990年 | 24篇 |
1989年 | 26篇 |
1988年 | 23篇 |
1987年 | 22篇 |
1986年 | 33篇 |
1985年 | 34篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 27篇 |
1981年 | 20篇 |
1980年 | 19篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1978年 | 27篇 |
1976年 | 24篇 |
1975年 | 20篇 |
1974年 | 22篇 |
1973年 | 19篇 |
1972年 | 19篇 |
1971年 | 16篇 |
1970年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有1682条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Bernard Molyneux 《No?s (Detroit, Mich.)》2009,43(1):116-136
The transparency argument concludes that we're directly aware of external properties and not directly aware of the properties of experience. Focusing on the presentation used by Michael Tye (2002) I contend that the argument requires experience to have content that it cannot plausibly have. I attribute the failure to a faulty account of the transparency phenomenon and conclude by suggesting an alternative understanding that is independently plausible, is not an error‐theory and yet renders the transparency of experience compatible with mental‐paint style views. 相似文献
102.
The purpose of the study was to investigate occupational stress among Chinese factory workers (N=342), from three cities of South East China, using the shortened version of the Occupational Stress Indicator (OSI)-2. The
results showed that the reliabilities and predictive validity of the OSI-2 subscales and other subscales used for the study
were reasonably high. Quite a high percentage of workers perceived high work pressure. The main sources of stress were intrinsic
to the job; and the coping strategies that were most frequently used to tackle stress were “control” methods. In addition,
“satisfaction on environmental condition” seemed to be a common predictor for job satisfaction, and mental and physical well-being.
The logical relationships between job satisfaction, mental well-being and physical well-being in Chinese workers have provided
support to the findings obtained in Western countries. 相似文献
103.
104.
Eric F. Rietzschel Bernard A. Nijstad Wolfgang Stroebe 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2010,101(1):47-68
It is commonly assumed that successful innovation depends on creative idea generation: the more ideas are generated, the higher the probability of selecting a very good idea should be. However, research has shown that people do not perform optimally at idea selection and that ideational output may not contribute much to creative idea selection. The present studies aim to explain this phenomenon. We identified the strong tendency of our participants to select feasible and desirable ideas, at the cost of originality, as the main reason for their poor selection performance. Two manipulations of participants' processing of the available ideas (exclusion instructions and quality ratings) had no effect on selection effectiveness. In contrast, explicitly instructing participants to select creative or original ideas did improve selection effectiveness with regard to idea originality, but at the same time decreased participants' satisfaction and the rated effectiveness of chosen ideas. Results are discussed in relation to an effectiveness‐originality trade off. 相似文献
105.
Nathalie Nallet Marlène Bernard Blandine Gadegbeku Karine Supernant Mireille Chiron 《Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour》2010,13(2):92-105
A self-administered questionnaire was answered by 2014 drivers, 853 of whom were taking a course. The data were analyzed aiming to identify the factors that are linked to taking the course, i.e. to recurrent reoffending.The course-takers, of whom 89% were male, admitted committing more violations than the control group. They had more accidents. More male course-takers than controls reported not being happy, and they more frequently had high extroversion scores.The multivariate analysis showed that, among men, taking the course was associated with the mileage, the feeling of being almost certain to be charged for an offence and having a high risk of losing one’s licence, in addition to being young, having a large number of brothers and sisters, higher socio-occupational category and smoking. Among women, in spite of the small number of them on the course, the mileage, the feeling of having a high risk of losing one’s licence, driving alone, the mental representation of their own vehicle as an extension of their office, and defining themselves as liking to take risks during leisure activities were significantly linked to taking a points recovery course.Those taking the course were not therefore doing so by chance; they differed from the general population of drivers, which was also precisely described with regard to driving habits and representations.Recommendations are then made in order to redirect the content of courses more towards introspection than technical matters. 相似文献
106.
Bernard Guerney Jr. 《Contemporary Family Therapy》1985,7(1):40-49
This report summarizes the results of a nearly day-long session of a group of experienced and distinguished family therapy practitioners and researchers. The task the group set for itself was to begin delineating the most important variables to attend to in understanding family therapy and evaluating its effectiveness. With full recognition that such a task could be only partially and tentatively accomplished in the time allotted, a great many such variables were outlined. 相似文献
107.
An assessment of the Hasher and Zacks (1979) conceptualization of the automatic nature of the acquisition of spatial location information was made in the context of manipulations of subjects' intention to learn and mood state. The Velten (1968) mood induction procedure was used to establish depressed and nondepressed subjects. Half of each group was then presented with a matrix of geometric forms under instructions to learn the location of the forms (intentional) or to evaluate the pleasantness of the array (incidental). Subsequent tests of spatial location performance found no differences as a function of subjects' mood state, but did find a strong effect for instructional condition. The superior performance of the intentional subjects raises questions about the automatic nature of the encoding of spatial location. 相似文献
108.
The attitudes of 674 speech-language pathologists toward stuttering, stutterers, stuttering therapy, parents of stutterers, and related issues were studied during the years 1973–1983. During that period, clinician attitudes were found to shift away from support of the John-sonian concepts, which suggest parental causality and dangers in early intervention. In addition, clinicians became less likely to perceive stutterers as possessing psychologic disorders and misperceptions of their problem and of their interpersonal relationships. However, a significant number of clinician's were found to continue to hold unsubstantiated beliefs regarding the personality of stutterers, their parents, and the efficacy of early intervention with very young stutterers. The results of the study were interpreted as suggesting the need to educate fluency specialists. 相似文献
109.
An attempt was made to demonstrate that in spite of the clinically documented characteristic inverse relationship between fantasy activity and physical activity, there also exists a direct, associative relationship between the two. M and other inkblot scores were the measures of fantasy, and inkblot responses were obtained before and during exercise (not after exercise, as previous studies). A significant increase was observed in the inkblot measures during exercise, and this associative bond between fantasy and physical activity was seen in the light of: 1) interference with the organism-environment interaction; 2) the dual meaning of M; and 3) the development of ego delay capacity. 相似文献
110.
Bernard Susser 《Inquiry (Oslo, Norway)》2013,56(3):245-260
My objective in the following study is to present and analyze the objections to the ‘classical argument’ in the sociology of knowledge raised by Leo Strauss and Karl Popper. Building on this expository account, I will attempt to demonstrate (1) that the opposition of Strauss and Popper is more apparent and polemical than real, (2) that the position taken by Strauss and Popper on the viability of a sociology of knowledge is essentially no different from that taken by the discipline's founders: Max Scheler, Karl Mannheim, and Georg Lukács, (3) that whatever the merits of Strauss and Popper's contentions, they become relevant only in the context of a radicalized version of the sociology of knowledge which developed subsequent to their formulation, and (4) that the sociology of knowledge has needlessly and deplorably become the battleground for meta‐theoretical disputes that are irrelevant to its practice. 相似文献