全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1724篇 |
免费 | 54篇 |
专业分类
1778篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 15篇 |
2023年 | 13篇 |
2022年 | 12篇 |
2021年 | 35篇 |
2020年 | 48篇 |
2019年 | 53篇 |
2018年 | 65篇 |
2017年 | 84篇 |
2016年 | 89篇 |
2015年 | 72篇 |
2014年 | 73篇 |
2013年 | 236篇 |
2012年 | 130篇 |
2011年 | 99篇 |
2010年 | 57篇 |
2009年 | 66篇 |
2008年 | 73篇 |
2007年 | 66篇 |
2006年 | 54篇 |
2005年 | 38篇 |
2004年 | 37篇 |
2003年 | 32篇 |
2002年 | 25篇 |
2001年 | 19篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 12篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 12篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 13篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 14篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 7篇 |
1970年 | 6篇 |
1967年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有1778条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
931.
Cotton top tamarins were tested in visible and invisible displacement tasks in a method similar to that used elsewhere to
test squirrel monkeys and orangutans. All subjects performed at levels significantly above chance on visible (n=8) and invisible (n=7) displacements, wherein the tasks included tests of the perseverance error, tests of memory in double and triple displacements,
and "catch" trials that tested for the use of the experimenter's hand as a cue for the correct cup. Performance on all nine
tasks was significantly higher than chance level selection of cups, and tasks using visible displacements generated more accurate
performance than tasks using invisible displacements. Performance was not accounted for by a practice effect based on exposure
to successive tasks. Results suggest that tamarins possess stage 6 object permanence capabilities, and that in a situation
involving brief exposure to tasks and foraging opportunities, tracking objects' movements and responding more flexibly are
abilities expressed readily by the tamarins.
Electronic Publication 相似文献
932.
933.
Owen Richard Lightsey Jr. Richelle McGhee Audrey Ervin George Gharibian Gharghani Eli Benjamin Rarey Rosaire Patrick Daigle Katherine Frances Wright Donnalin Constantin Kevin Powell 《Journal of Happiness Studies》2013,14(1):1-18
Life satisfaction is an important index of mental health and also predicts other important outcomes such as longevity and decreased mortality. Negative affect has a unique inverse relationship with life satisfaction across the life span. Therefore, determining psychological factors that uniquely predict future life satisfaction and that reduce the trait negative affect—life satisfaction relationship is theoretically and clinically important. In light of recent evidence from long-term longitudinal studies that self-efficacy for regulation of negative emotions (SERN) predicts higher future life satisfaction, as well as evidence from a cross-sectional study that a subtype of SERN—self-efficacy for regulating anger—buffers the relationship between trait negative affect and life satisfaction, we tested whether SERN and subtypes of SERN predicted higher life satisfaction and buffered the negative affect—life satisfaction relationship longitudinally over short time periods. After controlling for time 1 life satisfaction, higher time 1 self-efficacy for regulating despondency and distress (SEDes) predicted higher future life satisfaction over average time periods of 17 days (N = 127), 32 days (N = 83), and 41 days (N = 65) among college students. However, in post hoc exploratory regressions that included self-efficacy for experience and expression of positive emotions (SEPos), SEDes predicted higher time 2 life satisfaction, but only SEPos uniquely predicted higher time 3 and time 4 life satisfaction. 相似文献
934.
Positive Automatic Cognitions Mediate the Relationship Between Personality and Trait Positive Affect
Owen Richard Lightsey Jr. George Gharibian Gharghani Aron Matthew Katz Valerie Ann McKinney Eli Benjamin Rarey 《Journal of Happiness Studies》2013,14(1):115-134
Positive affect (PA) has emerged as a key well-being facet and a powerful predictor of physical and psychological well-being. One of the strongest predictors of PA is personality, particularly neuroticism and extraversion. However, the mechanisms via which personality affects PA are not well-understood. Consistent with theories that accord cognitive factors a role in mediating the relationship between personality and outcomes, we tested whether positive automatic thoughts (PATs) mediated the relationship between neuroticism, extraversion, and PA among 199 college students (137 women, 70% White, 66% first and second year students, mean age = 24.13, SD = 8.86). Consistent with hypotheses, structural equation modeling indicated that PATs fully mediated the relationship between both neuroticism and extraversion and PA. The structural model fit the data well, χ2/df = 2.33, CFI = .96, SRMR = .07, RMSEA = .08 (90% CI: .06, .10), AIC = 172.45, and accounted for 58% of the variance in PA. An alternative model in which personality predicted PA, which in turn predicted PATs, did not provide as good a fit to the data, χ2/df = 3.03, CFI = .94, SRMR = .09, RMSEA = .10 (90% CI: .08, .12), AIC = 207.40. Models in which negative automatic thoughts (NATs) were construed as fully [χ2/df = 4.46, CFI = .95, SRMR = .08, RMSEA = .13 (90% CI: .11, .16)] or partially [χ2/df = 4.04, CFI = .96, SRMR = .06, RMSEA = .12 (90% CI: .10, .15)] mediating the relationship between neuroticism and negative affect did not provide a good fit to the data. A final model in which negative affect was tested a mediator of the relationship between neuroticism and NATs also did not fit the data well, χ2/df = 4.03, CFI = .96, SRMR = .07, RMSEA = .12 (90% CI: .10, .15). 相似文献
935.
Dyson BJ Quinlan PT 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2004,30(6):1117-1131
In 3 experiments, the authors tested performance in simple tone matching and classification tasks. Each tone was defined on location and frequency dimensions. In the first 2 experiments, participants completed a same-different matching task on the basis of one of these dimensions while attempting to ignore irrelevant variation in the other dimension. In Experiment 3, in which the tones were classified either by frequency or location, the authors explored intertrial repetition effects. The patterns of performance across these different tasks were remarkably similar and were taken to reveal basic characteristics of stimulus encoding processes. The data suggest a processing sequence in audition that reveals an early stage in which location and frequency are treated as being integral and a latter stage in which location and frequency are separable. 相似文献
936.
This study assessed the factor structure of the Questionnaire on Smoking Urges (QSU), a commonly used assessment of cravings for cigarettes, with a sample of smokers presenting for treatment in a smoking cessation trial. On the basis of previous research, three confirmatory factor analytic models were tested. Model 1 hypothesized a 26-item, 2-factor model using the items reported in the original QSU analysis by S. T. Tiffany and D. J. Drobes (1991). Model 2 hypothesized a 12-item, 2-factor model comprised of the 6 most robust items found in each of the 2 factors of the original factor analysis. Using the 12 items from Model 2, Model 3 hypothesized a 12-item, 1-factor model. The 2nd model was found to fit the data best. Reliability was also tested using values obtained in this 2nd model, and these estimates were found to be reasonably good. Future research directions for the QSU are discussed. 相似文献
937.
Loeber R Green SM Lahey BB Frick PJ McBurnett K 《Clinical child and family psychology review》2000,3(1):37-60
The paper summarizes the first decade of the Developmental Trends Study, a prospective longitudinal study of 177 boys. Initially, they were referred to mental health clinics in Pennsylvania (Pittsburgh), and Georgia (Athens and Atlanta). Since 1987, the boys, their parents, and their teachers have been followed up almost annually. The study is unique because the cooperation rate of participants has remained very high over the years, psychiatric diagnoses were derived from structured interviews (especially disruptive behavior disorders), and many risk factors were measured over the years. The present paper summarizes key findings on the development of disruptive behavior, especially Oppositional Defiant Disorder, Conduct Disorder, and Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder. The paper also highlights results on risk factors and comorbid conditions of disruptive behaviors. 相似文献
938.
Investigations of the ways in which the eyes move came to prominence in the 19th century, but techniques for measuring them more precisely emerged in the 20th century. When scanning a scene or text the eyes engage in periods of relative stability (fixations) interspersed with ballistic rotations (saccades). The saccade-and-fixate strategy, associated with voluntary eye movements, was first uncovered in the context of involuntary eye movements following body rotation. This pattern of eye movements is now referred to as nystagmus, and involves periods of slow eye movements, during which objects are visible, and rapid returns, when they are not; it is based on a vestibular reflex which attempts to achieve image stabilisation. Post-rotational nystagmus was reported in the late 18th century (by Wells), with afterimages used as a means of retinal stabilisation to distinguish between movement of the eyes and of the environment. Nystagmus was linked to vestibular stimulation in the 19th century, and Mach, Breuer, and Crum Brown all described its fast and slow phases. Wells and Breuer proposed that there was no visual awareness during the ballistic phase (saccadic suppression). The saccade-and-fixate strategy highlighted by studies of nystagmus was shown to apply to tasks like reading by Dodge, who used more sophisticated photographic techniques to examine oculomotor kinematics. The relationship between eye movements and perception, following earlier intuitions by Wells and Breuer, was explored by Dodge, and has been of fundamental importance in the direction of vision research over the last century. 相似文献
939.
Studies in change blindness re-enforce the suggestion that veridical, pictorial representations that survive multiple relocations of gaze are unlikely to be generated in the visual system. However, more abstract information may well be extracted and represented by the visual system. In this paper we study the types of information that are retained and the time courses over which these representations are constructed when participants view complex natural scenes. We find that such information is retained and that the resultant abstract representations encode a range of information. Different types of information are extracted and represented over different time courses. After several seconds of viewing natural scenes, our visual system is able to construct a complex information-rich representation. 相似文献
940.
Stressful life experiences have been related to emotional distress, but whether different classes of stressful events are associated with different emotional reactions has not been assessed. In this study, 34 college students completed the Inventory of College Students' Recent Life Experiences, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, and Beck Depression Inventory. Consistent with previous research, most classes of daily hassles were significantly related to both depression and anxiety. The nonsignificant difference for one class of life experience, Friendship Problems, to be more strongly associated with anxiety than depression should be studied carefully. 相似文献