全文获取类型
收费全文 | 98篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
100篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 1篇 |
2019年 | 3篇 |
2016年 | 1篇 |
2015年 | 2篇 |
2014年 | 2篇 |
2013年 | 12篇 |
2012年 | 7篇 |
2011年 | 4篇 |
2010年 | 5篇 |
2009年 | 3篇 |
2008年 | 5篇 |
2007年 | 4篇 |
2006年 | 2篇 |
2005年 | 3篇 |
2004年 | 1篇 |
2003年 | 1篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1939年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有100条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
In this study we evaluated the effects of two psychoeducational groups (anger management and goal setting) on state and trait
anger, anger expression, and on self-efficacy beliefs of high risk adolescents enrolled in an alternative high school. Participants
(n = 57) were pre- and post-tested using the State Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI) with subscales for (a) state anger,
(b) trait anger, and (c) anger expression, and on a measure of self-efficacy related to (a) anger management, (b) goal setting,
and (c) group behavior and interpersonal communication. We hypothesized that each treatment would favorably impact the self-efficacy
items related to it, that participants in both treatment groups would show gains in self-efficacy for general group behavior
and communication skills, and that the anger management treatment would decrease state anger and anger expression. Results
of a series of Repeated Measures ANOVAS revealed no group effects or group by time interaction effects. Analysis of Cohen's
d effect sizes for the treatment groups in comparison with the control condition indicated only small treatment effects for
the goal setting group on the STAXI, and small, negative treatment effects for the anger management group on the STAXI and
on state anger. Thus, results failed to support the hypotheses with the current data set. Limitations of this study and implications
for future research and group practice within schools are discussed.
The authors thank Ellen Hawley McWhirter, Greg Sandstrom, and the administration, faculty, and students of the participating
“alternative high school” for their help and support. 相似文献
72.
73.
There has been a recent trend in research seeking the most appropriate statistical technique for determining job similarities/differences. Monte Carlo methods were used to analyze more closely the repeated measures analysis of variance and the multivariate analysis of variance in order to add further insight into the viability of these techniques for this purpose. The conventional univariate analysis of variance, the ε-adjusted univariate F test, and the ε-adjusted univariate F test were compared to three multivariate tests (Roy's largest-root criterion, Wilk's likelihood ratio, and the Pillai-Barlett trace) in terms of power and control for Type I error when (1) circularity and homogeneity were met, (2) homogeneity was met but circularity was violated, (3) homogeneity was violated but circularity was met, and (4) both homogeneity and circularity were violated. The efficacy of the techniques was shown to be contingent upon whether the assumptions were met or not. The univariate test proved to be the better technique when circularity was met. The multivariate technique proved to be the better test when homogeneity was met while circularity was violated. The results were mixed when both circularity and homogeneity were violated. Guidelines for selecting a statistical technique which tests for job differences are offered. 相似文献
74.
Anthony C. Little Benedict C. Jones Lisa M. DeBruine Robin I.M. Dunbar 《Personality and individual differences》2013
People readily ascribe personality traits to others and believe that faces hold important guides to character. Here we examined the relationship between static facial appearance and self-reported cooperation/defection using the prisoner’s dilemma (N = 193). Study 1 combined face images of those self-reporting they would be most and least likely to cooperate. The composites of cooperators were seen as more cooperative than non-cooperators. Study 2 demonstrated accuracy with ratings of individual faces. Masculinity of face shape was negatively related to self-reported cooperation for men, but not women. Further, ratings of smile intensity were positively, but not significantly, related to self-reported cooperation. Overall, individuals appear able judge the potential of others to cooperate from static facial appearance alone at rates greater than chance. 相似文献
75.
Dissociating averageness and attractiveness: attractive faces are not always average 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
DeBruine LM Jones BC Unger L Little AC Feinberg DR 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2007,33(6):1420-1430
Although the averageness hypothesis of facial attractiveness proposes that the attractiveness of faces is mostly a consequence of their averageness, 1 study has shown that caricaturing highly attractive faces makes them mathematically less average but more attractive. Here the authors systematically test the averageness hypothesis in 5 experiments using both rating and visual adaptation paradigms. Visual adaptation has previously been shown to increase both preferences for previously viewed face types (i.e., attractiveness) and their perceived normality (i.e., averageness). The authors used a visual adaptation procedure to test whether facial attractiveness is dependent upon faces' proximity to average (averageness hypothesis) or their location relative to average along an attractiveness dimension in face space (contrast hypothesis). While the typical pattern of change due to visual adaptation was found for judgments of normality, judgments of attractiveness resulted in a very different pattern. The results of these 5 experiments conclusively support the proposal that there are specific nonaverage characteristics that are particularly attractive. The authors discuss important implications for the interpretation of studies using a visual adaptation paradigm to investigate attractiveness. 相似文献
76.
This article describes a methodology for the capture and analysis of hybrid data. A case study in the field of reasoning with
multiple representations—specifically, in computer programming—is presented to exemplify the use of the methodology. The hybrid
data considered comprise computer interaction logs, audio recordings, and data about visual attention focus. The capture of
the focus of visual attention data is performed with software. The software employed tracks the user’s visual attention by
blurring parts of the stimuli presented on the screen and allowing the participant to see only a small region of it at any
one time. These hybrid data are analyzed via a methodology that combines qualitative and quantitative approaches. The article
describes the software tool employed and the analytic methodology, and also discusses data capture issues and limitations
of the approach. 相似文献
77.
From a functionalist perspective, human memory should be attuned to information of adaptive value for one’s survival and reproductive
fitness. While evidence of sensitivity to survival-related information is growing, specific links between memory and information
that could impact upon reproductive fitness have remained elusive. Here, in two experiments, we showed that memory in women
is sensitive to male voice pitch, a sexually dimorphic cue important for mate choice because it not only serves as an indicator
of genetic quality, but may also signal behavioural traits undesirable in a long-term partner. In Experiment 1, we report that women’s visual object memory is significantly enhanced when an object’s name is spoken during encoding in
a masculinised (i.e., lower-pitch) versus feminised (i.e., higher-pitch) male voice, but that no analogous effect occurs when
women listen to other women’s voices. Experiment 2 replicated this pattern of results, additionally showing that lowering and raising male voice pitch enhanced and impaired
women’s memory, respectively, relative to a baseline (i.e., unmanipulated) voice condition. The modulatory effect of sexual
dimorphism cues in the male voice may reveal a mate-choice adaptation within women’s memory, sculpted by evolution in response
to the dilemma posed by the double-edged qualities of male masculinity. 相似文献
78.
PJ Fraccaro DR Feinberg LM Debruine AC Little CD Watkins BC Jones 《Evolutionary psychology》2010,8(3):447-461
Sexually dimorphic physical traits are important for mate choice and mate preference in many species, including humans. Several previous studies have observed that women's preferences for physical cues of male masculinity in different domains (e.g., visual and vocal) are correlated. These correlations demonstrate systematic, rather than arbitrary, variation in women's preferences for masculine men and are consistent with the proposal that sexually dimorphic cues in different domains reflect a common underlying aspect of male quality. Here we present evidence for a similar correlation between men's preferences for different cues of femininity in women; although men generally preferred feminized to masculinized versions of both women's faces and voices, the strength of men's preferences for feminized versions of female faces was positively and significantly correlated with the strength of their preferences for feminized versions of women's voices. In a second study, this correlation occurred when men judged women's attractiveness as long-term, but not short-term, mates, which is consistent with previous research. Collectively, these findings (1) present novel evidence for systematic variation in men's preferences for feminine women, (2) present converging evidence for concordant preferences for sexually dimorphic traits in different domains, and (3) complement findings of correlations between women's facial and vocal femininity. 相似文献
79.
Frank J. Penedo Catherine Benedict Eric S. Zhou Mikal Rasheed Lara Traeger Bruce R. Kava Mark Soloway Sara Czaja Michael H. Antoni 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2013,20(1):25-32
Advanced prostate cancer (APC) is associated with disruptions that compromise health related quality of life (HRQOL). Treatment often includes androgendeprivation therapy (ADT), which results in a range of side effects (e.g., fatigue, urinary dysfunction) that further impact HRQOL. Despite these challenges, there are limited evaluations of the impact of stress and stress management skills on HRQOL among APC survivors on ADT. This study evaluated relationships among stress, stress management skills, and HRQOL, and it was hypothesized that better stress management skills would relate to greater physical and emotional well-being by mitigating perceived stress levels. Participants (N = 77) were 69.7 years old (SD = 9.8), 18.6 months post-treatment (SD = 17.5), and ethnically diverse (65 % Non-Hispanic White, 13 % Hispanic, 21 % African-American). Measures included the Measure of Current Status for stress management skills, the Perceived Stress Scale for perceived stress, and the Medical Outcomes Study—Short Form (MOS SF-36; physical functioning and emotional well-being subscales) for HRQOL. Direct effects and mediation models were evaluated to determine the relationships between perceived stress, stress management skills, and HRQOL domains, controlling for relevant covariates. Stress management skills and perceived stress were significantly associated with physical functioning (β = .24, p < .05 and β = ?.43, p < .01, respectively) and emotional well-being (β = .35, p < .01 and β = ?.64, p < .01, respectively). Regression analyses supported the hypothesis that reduced perceived stress mediated the relationship between stress management skills and both physical functioning and emotional well-being. These results demonstrate that one way stress management skills may impact HRQOL is by lessening ongoing perceptions of stress. 相似文献
80.
Few studies have investigated how physical and social facial cues are integrated in the formation of face preferences. Here we show that expression differentially qualifies the strength of attractiveness preferences for faces with direct and averted gaze. For judgments of faces with direct gaze, attractiveness preferences were stronger for smiling faces than for faces with neutral expressions. By contrast, for judgments of faces with averted gaze, attractiveness preferences were stronger for faces with neutral expressions than for smiling faces. Because expressions can differ in meaning depending on whether they are directed toward or away from oneself, it is only by integrating gaze direction, facial expression, and physical attractiveness that one can unambiguously identify the most attractive individuals who are likely to reciprocate one's own social interest. 相似文献