首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   722篇
  免费   26篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2023年   6篇
  2022年   8篇
  2021年   10篇
  2020年   14篇
  2019年   30篇
  2018年   18篇
  2017年   21篇
  2016年   33篇
  2015年   12篇
  2014年   31篇
  2013年   76篇
  2012年   47篇
  2011年   34篇
  2010年   26篇
  2009年   21篇
  2008年   27篇
  2007年   26篇
  2006年   24篇
  2005年   20篇
  2004年   16篇
  2003年   15篇
  2002年   10篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   10篇
  1993年   6篇
  1991年   5篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   9篇
  1988年   7篇
  1987年   8篇
  1985年   7篇
  1983年   5篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   6篇
  1978年   4篇
  1977年   6篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   5篇
  1973年   5篇
  1972年   7篇
  1971年   7篇
  1970年   5篇
  1968年   9篇
  1967年   7篇
  1966年   5篇
  1964年   5篇
  1963年   4篇
  1958年   5篇
排序方式: 共有750条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
102.
103.
We revived Jung’s Archetypes to characterize what we see as an emerging confluence of evolutionary, embodied, and ecological responses to traditional cognitive models of mental representation. We propose that all humans possess archetypal representational systems that are (a) computationally grounded in perception and action, (b) shaped by learning and culture, and (c) biologically prepared by our ancestral past. Because these functionally modular systems arose in tandem with—and in response to—our increasingly complex social world, one implication is that even the most abstract issues studied in cognitive science may be, at least in part, scaffolded on more ancient social and emotional calculi.  相似文献   
104.
105.
Parents can influence their children’s problematic mobile phone involvement (PMPI) by engaging in parental mediation activities, such as restrictions or co-use, by being a role model, and by their general and domain-unspecific parenting style that facilitates more or less attachment security of the child. This study tested the associations between these different routes of influence—parental mediation of the mobile phone, parental PMPI, and secure child-parent attachment—with children’s PMPI. Data was acquired from a quota-sample survey with 500 children, between 8 and 14 years of age, and one of their parents. Results point to the importance of open and empathic parent-child-communication, as well as a positive relationship quality, and demonstrate the detrimental effects of parents’ own PMPI on children’s PMPI.  相似文献   
106.
Determined by anatomical characteristics, the binocular visual field has an elliptical shape. Whereas the focus of attention – that area of the visual field in which objects can be consciously perceived – is significantly smaller, its shape is not necessarily dependent on the same factors. Indeed, it is yet unknown if the maximum extents of the attentional focus’ meridians are dimensionally stable or adapt to the egocentric perspective. In this study, we intended to measure the expansion up to which peripheral stimuli can still be perceived while participants were sitting and lying on the left and on the right body side. Results demonstrate that contrary to the visual field the maximum extents along the attentional focus’ meridians remain stable across different head positions with a greater horizontal than vertical alignment. This indicates that the expansion ratios of the meridians of the attentional focus are not dependent on the egocentric perspective. Rather, they appear stable in space and gravity-based. Findings are discussed in terms of our visual environment and current as well as alternative interpretations are weighed against each other.  相似文献   
107.
108.
Weighted‐additive (WADD) strategies require decision makers to integrate multiple values weighted by their relevance. From what age can children make choices in line with such a WADD‐strategy? We compare multi‐attribute decisions of children (6–7; 8–10; 11–12‐year‐olds) with adults in an open information‐board environment without pre‐decisional information search. In two experiments, we classify decision strategies based on individual choice patterns and find that in all age groups the majority of participants are users of a WADD‐strategy. Simple decision heuristics such as lexicographic strategies were applied rarely by children and not at all by adults. In two additional follow‐up studies, we further investigate the underlying process of WADD‐application by analysing decision latencies in combination with a retrospective think‐aloud study. Results suggest that children did not apply WADD‐strategies in a deliberate fashion in our experiments. Overall, our findings demonstrate that the ability to make good and quick decisions by holistically integrating information is already present in young children. Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
109.
In this paper, condom use or non‐use in sexual intercourse with casual partners is analyzed on the basis of ideas derived from the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen & Madden, 1986), which is combined with the general theory of crime (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). An integrated model allows the assumption that persons who display low self‐control are likely to seek out opportunities to engage in sexual intercourse with casual partners and are likely to display attitudes and subjective norms favoring the non‐use of condoms. The study consists of an empirical examination of these assumptions. A standardized questionnaire (vignette technique) was presented to a quota sample of 837 German adults aged 18 to 65 years. Loglinear analyses were carried out to examine the integrative model. The results are discussed with regard to the effectiveness of the theoretical framework applied and with regard to previous research that has been conducted in this area.  相似文献   
110.
In the period of 1988–1999 752 anamnestic reports made by psychoanalysts-in-training on the basis of initial interviews with treatment seeking patients were examined concerning their psychodynamical judgement criteria. The study was conducded at the Institut für Psychotherapie e.V. in Berlin. Trained raters analysed first the presented “psychodynamic considerations” concerning their implicit theoretical and clinical concepts and second the supervising analysts commentaries. Moreover, the supervising analysts were asked to explicate their own theoretical orientation. The findings were then put in comparison. In the course of the study, changes over time in preferences for some concepts and diagnoses were observable. Psychoanalysts-in-training differ from experienced psychoanalysts in their preferred data sources, and men and women differ in their diagnoses. Concerning their theoretical orientation, psychoanalysts-in-training lean on their instructors.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号