全文获取类型
收费全文 | 987篇 |
免费 | 23篇 |
专业分类
1010篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2023年 | 7篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 20篇 |
2020年 | 29篇 |
2019年 | 40篇 |
2018年 | 42篇 |
2017年 | 42篇 |
2016年 | 37篇 |
2015年 | 36篇 |
2014年 | 35篇 |
2013年 | 114篇 |
2012年 | 45篇 |
2011年 | 44篇 |
2010年 | 48篇 |
2009年 | 46篇 |
2008年 | 39篇 |
2007年 | 30篇 |
2006年 | 29篇 |
2005年 | 34篇 |
2004年 | 34篇 |
2003年 | 23篇 |
2002年 | 21篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 13篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有1010条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
Preservation and loss to forgetting of autobiographical memories is a focus in both the adult and developmental literatures. In both, there are comparative arguments regarding rates of forgetting. Children are assumed to forget autobiographical memories more rapidly than adults, and younger children are assumed to forget more rapidly than older children. Yet few studies can directly inform these comparisons: few feature children and adults, and few prospectively track the survival of specific autobiographical memories over time. In a 4-year prospective study, we obtained autobiographical memories from children 4, 6, and 8 years, and adults. We tested recall of different subsets of the events after 1, 2, and 3 years. Accelerated rates of forgetting were apparent among all child groups relative to adults; within the child groups, 4- and 6-year-olds had accelerated forgetting relative to 8-year-olds. The differences were especially pronounced in open-ended recall. The thematic coherence of initial memory reports also was a significant predictor of the survival of specific memories. The pattern of findings is consistent with suggestions that the adult distribution of autobiographical memories is achieved as the quality of memory traces increases (here measured by thematic coherence) and the rate of forgetting decreases. 相似文献
152.
Islamic banking is based on moral foundations that make it distinct from conventional banking. Some argue that because of its foundation in Islam, Islamic banking may represent a more morally appealing alternative. Yet, evidence shows that this is not the case. Indeed, the current practice of Islamic banking has not been able to achieve its goals which are based on Islam's moral values: to enhance justice, equitability, and social well‐being. This essay examines the extent to which Islamic banking is ethical and concludes that the practice of the industry does not seem to be de facto ethical from the Islamic perspective of ethical values. It only consists in trading the same instruments of conventional banks without genuinely enforcing Islam's ethical vision. The practice of Islamic banking misrepresents Islam and does not contribute to solving social problems. The interaction between maqasid al‐shari?a (objectives of Islamic law) and qiyās (deductive analogy) provides a supplementary tool for interpreting the failure of the prior in terms of the practical misuse of the latter by Islamic banks. This essay provides an interpretive approach to the current debate about why Islamic banking has failed and suggests ways to move cautiously in the future. 相似文献
153.
154.
155.
Firms allocate considerable resources to test job applicants' skills and to ensure that hiring decisions are nondiscriminatory. Interpreting selection output in light of preliminary information may undermine the impartiality of personnel selection decisions. In this study, human resource managers were presented preliminary information about a candidate's performance and asked to rate him according to a detailed assessment‐center report of his performance. We examined several interventions for reducing reliance on preliminary information: retrieving assessment‐center information, generating a rating model, or both. Participants (N = 167) excluded preliminary information from the interventions, but relied on it when rating the candidate. The documented failure to control for the effects of preliminary information on subsequent judgments can contribute to improved selection procedures. 相似文献
156.
隔代抚育中出现了普遍的隔代“亲”现象。本文分别从文化传统(血缘宗亲)、心理需求、社会现实、补偿心理等几个方面来阐述隔代“亲”的成因。 相似文献
157.
R. Elliott Ingersoll Ann Bauer Laura Burns 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2004,82(3):337-343
There are currently millions of children with mental and emotional symptoms who are being treated with psychotropic medication. The authors critique such treatment and suggest that advocacy counseling is a responsible approach to balance the power of both the pharmaceutical companies and the medical model in the mental health arena. 相似文献
158.
Patricia J. Bauer Dana L. van Abbema Sandra A. Wiebe Mari Strand Cary Christina Phill Melissa M. Burch 《Applied cognitive psychology》2004,18(4):373-392
When they are tested nonverbally, even young children demonstrate long-term recall. There have been few studies of whether early memories later are verbally accessible; the results of those that exist are mixed. Inconsistencies may be due to differences in the contextual cues provided at the time of recall. In two experiments, children 13–20 months were exposed to multi-step sequences and tested for nonverbal recall after 3–6 months. At age 3 years, they were tested verbally, under varying conditions of contextual support: in the original laboratory with event-related props versus at home with photographs of the props (Experiment 1), and at home with props (Experiment 2). Children younger than 20 months at initial experience of the events did not demonstrate verbal recall. Children who were 20 months at the initial exposure recalled verbally, as long as they had physical props as cues, regardless of whether testing took place at home or in the laboratory. This research informs the conditions under which memories from very early childhood later can be recalled verbally. Copyright © 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
159.
Parents need to be supported, valued, and educated to contribute to the healthy sexual development of their children. The authors present a rationale for counselors to guide parents as primary sex educators for their children through collaboration with counselors, administrators, and parents. 相似文献
160.
Melissa Burch Amina Jaafar Tiffany Weigle West Patricia Bauer 《Memory (Hove, England)》2013,21(5):517-529
To examine the impact of early linguistic experiences on later verbal report of autobiographical memory, 13 hearing adults and 13 deaf adults born to hearing parents described events that occurred before and after the age of 10 years. The contextual, temporal, and thematic coherence of the narratives was rated. The use of emotional, perceptual, mental, and physiological states was also recorded. There were differences in the coherence of the narratives and use of internal states according to the age at which the events occurred. There were no group differences in coherence, but hearing adults provided longer narratives than deaf adults. When narrative length was controlled, deaf adults included more emotional states than hearing adults. Results suggest that early unavailability of language does not impact the coherence of adults’ narratives, although certain features of linguistic expression specific to ASL may result in greater saturation of emotional states references in autobiographical narratives of deaf adults. 相似文献