首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   853篇
  免费   19篇
  国内免费   7篇
  2021年   8篇
  2020年   10篇
  2019年   11篇
  2018年   17篇
  2017年   8篇
  2016年   18篇
  2014年   8篇
  2013年   45篇
  2012年   23篇
  2011年   13篇
  2010年   13篇
  2009年   11篇
  2008年   12篇
  2007年   22篇
  2006年   14篇
  2005年   13篇
  2004年   20篇
  2003年   9篇
  2002年   20篇
  2001年   35篇
  2000年   46篇
  1999年   20篇
  1998年   11篇
  1997年   8篇
  1992年   18篇
  1991年   15篇
  1990年   34篇
  1989年   23篇
  1988年   11篇
  1987年   23篇
  1986年   31篇
  1985年   21篇
  1984年   21篇
  1983年   16篇
  1982年   8篇
  1981年   10篇
  1980年   8篇
  1979年   12篇
  1978年   10篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   15篇
  1974年   15篇
  1973年   20篇
  1972年   14篇
  1971年   17篇
  1970年   15篇
  1969年   7篇
  1968年   9篇
  1967年   10篇
  1951年   6篇
排序方式: 共有879条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
741.
742.
In the current literature, empirical and conceptual distinctions have been drawn between a more or less passive short-term memory (STM) system and a more dynamic working memory (WM) system. Distinct tasks have been developed to measure their capacity and research has generally shown that, for adults, WM, and not STM, is a reliable predictor of general cognitive ability. However, the locus of the differences between the tasks has received little attention. We present data from children concerning measures of matrices reasoning ability, reading, and numerical skill along with forward and backward order serial recall of WM, STM, and STM with articulatory suppression tasks. As indices of children's cognitive skills, STM and WM are shown to be rather similar in terms of memory per se. Neither the opportunity for rehearsal nor task complexity provides satisfactory explanations for differences between memory tests.  相似文献   
743.
Conditioning the proboscis extension reflex of harnessed honeybees (Apis mellifera) is used to study the effect temporal spacing between successive conditioning trials has on memory. Retention is monitored at two long-term intervals corresponding to early (1 and 2 d after conditioning) and late long-term memory (3 and 4 d). The acquisition level is varied by using different conditioned stimuli (odors, mechanical stimulation, and temperature increase at the antenna), varying strengths of the unconditioned stimulus (sucrose), and various numbers of conditioning trials. How learning trials are spaced is the dominant factor both for acquisition and retention, and although longer intertrial intervals lead to better acquisition and higher retention, the level of acquisition per se does not determine the spacing effect on retention. Rather, spaced conditioning leads to higher memory consolidation both during acquisition and later, between the early and long-term memory phases. These consolidation processes can be selectively inhibited by blocking protein synthesis during acquisition.  相似文献   
744.
745.
U. Blau 《Erkenntnis》1986,25(2):177-196
Ohne Zusammenfassung  相似文献   
746.

Dislocation density studies have been performed on icosahedral Al-Pd-Mn single quasicrystals after plastic deformation and after subsequent heat treatment. The deformation tests were carried out at a constant strain rate of 10- 5 s-1 at temperatures between 695 and 820 C. The heat treatments were performed at 730 C, corresponding to one of the deformation temperatures. The development of the dislocation density during heat treatment and that during plastic deformation are compared. The experimental data are interpreted using a kinetic equation, which describes the evolution of the dislocation density during deformation. Numerical values for the dislocation multiplication constant and the annihilation rate for icosahedral Al-Pd-Mn are presented.  相似文献   
747.
We have combined high-angle annular dark field/scanning transmission electron microscopy (HAADF-STEM) tomography with bright field (BF)-TEM tomography to characterize small inclusions of Pb at a grain boundary in Al. It was found that the shape of the grain boundary inclusions is more complex than previously thought. By using moiré fringes observed at some orientations of the specimen in a BF-TEM tomographic tilt series, we were able to determine the orientation of each grain, the axis and angle of misorientation of the grain boundary, and the facet planes of the grain boundary inclusions. The 3D shape of the inclusions was determined by merging this information with the HAADF-STEM tomography.  相似文献   
748.
Information processing is a main property which distinguishes life from inorganic matter. Its background is the recognition activity of biomacro‐molecules based on quantum non‐demolition measurements. Reflective arrows in the set of mappings appearing from quantum measurements do form a hypercyclic structure which realizes information transfer and allows a system to self‐reproduce and evolve. These arrows correspond to Goedel numbers created inside a system.  相似文献   
749.
Hypnotic responding might be due to attenuated frontal lobe functioning after the hypnotic induction. Little is known about whether personality traits linked with frontal functioning are associated with responsiveness to hypnotic suggestions. We assessed whether hypnotic suggestibility is related to the traits of self-control and impulsivity in 154 participants who completed the Brief Self-Control Scale, the Self-Regulation Scale, the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11), and the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility (HGSHS:A). BIS-11 non-planning impulsivity correlated positively with HGSHS:A (Bonferroni-corrected). Furthermore, in the best model emerging from a stepwise multiple regression, both non-planning impulsivity and self-control positively predicted hypnotic suggestibility, and there was an interaction of BIS-11 motor impulsivity with gender. For men only, motor impulsivity tended to predict hypnotic suggestibility. Hypnotic suggestibility is associated with personality traits linked with frontal functioning, and hypnotic responding in men and women might differ.  相似文献   
750.
In 1944, with the Second World War all but lost for Germany and her bombed‐out industry unable to supply the military, one desperate measure taken to obtain needed war material was an offer by Eichmann to barter the lives of one million Jews for ten thousand trucks. The interaction between the SS and Jewish leaders in Budapest—and in turn their appeals outside Hungary—is the subject of five plays: one originating in Austria, two in Germany, one in Great Britain and one in Israel. All five are discussed here in terms of their approach to the subject, their raisons d’être and their pursuit of the ethical issues raised by Jewish involvement with the Nazis. Whether under a fictional or his real name, a central character in these plays is Reszö Kastner (1906–1957), who was accused after the war of cooperation with the Nazis.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号