首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   450篇
  免费   28篇
  478篇
  2020年   5篇
  2019年   7篇
  2018年   8篇
  2017年   13篇
  2016年   12篇
  2015年   7篇
  2014年   6篇
  2013年   44篇
  2012年   9篇
  2011年   18篇
  2010年   11篇
  2009年   14篇
  2008年   23篇
  2007年   21篇
  2006年   15篇
  2005年   11篇
  2004年   11篇
  2003年   13篇
  2002年   12篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   7篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   6篇
  1996年   5篇
  1995年   8篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   6篇
  1992年   9篇
  1991年   10篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   3篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   11篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   5篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   8篇
  1978年   5篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   5篇
  1974年   7篇
  1973年   5篇
  1972年   5篇
  1971年   4篇
  1969年   8篇
  1968年   10篇
  1967年   10篇
  1966年   5篇
排序方式: 共有478条查询结果,搜索用时 5 毫秒
471.
472.
Are people who report having forgotten their childhood sexual abuse (CSA) characterised by superior ability to inhibit retrieval of disturbing material? To test this hypothesis, we asked adults reporting either repressed, recovered, or continuous memories of CSA or no history of CSA to participate in a directed forgetting experiment (list method). They rated the emotionality of two consecutive lists of trauma‐related and positive words. After the first list, the experimenter instructed participants to forget these words, and to continue rating the remaining words. A surprise recall task revealed robust directed forgetting and valence effects: All groups recalled more words from the second list than from the first list, and recalled more trauma words than positive ones. Participants reporting either repressed or recovered memories of CSA did not exhibit superior forgetting of trauma versus positive words relative to the other two groups. Finally, a subsidiary analysis revealed that participants exhibiting a “repressor” coping style (low self‐reported anxiety plus high defensiveness) did not exhibit superior directed forgetting of trauma words.  相似文献   
473.
474.
In recent years, there has been increased attention to the development of “moral,” “social”, or “self-conscious” emotions, such as embarrassment, shame, and pride, in 2- and 3-year olds. In the present study, 17-month-olds' behaviours in several contexts were observed; and observations of behaviours of their parents were obtained. Results indicated that toddlers did react appropriately to the situations, and their behaviours in the semi-naturalistic situations cohered as three factors: a guilt factor, an embarrassment factor, and an anxiety/inhibition factor. Embarrassed behaviour included a smile accompanied by indications of embarrassment (gaze aversion, lip press, lip bite, and/or body touching/self-adaptors). Moreover, parental behaviours systematically predicted children's behaviour patterns, but self-recognition was unrelated to most behaviours. Implications and suggestions for further research are discussed.  相似文献   
475.
476.
477.
ABSTRACT

Theories of emotion have often maintained artificial boundaries: for instance, that cognition and emotion are separable, and that an emotion concept is separable from the emotional events that comprise its category (e.g. “fear” is distinct from instances of fear). Over the past several years, research has dissolved these artificial boundaries, suggesting instead that conceptual construction is a domain-general process—a process by which the brain makes meaning of the world. The brain constructs emotion concepts, but also cognitions and perceptions, all in the service of guiding action. In this view, concepts are multimodal constructions, dynamically prepared from a set of highly variable instances. This approach obviates old questions (e.g. how does cognition regulate emotion?) but generates new ones (e.g. how does a brain learn emotion concepts?). In this paper, we review this constructionist, predictive coding account of emotion, considering its implications for health and well-being, culture and development.  相似文献   
478.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号