首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   226篇
  免费   8篇
  234篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   4篇
  2019年   5篇
  2018年   2篇
  2017年   3篇
  2016年   2篇
  2015年   2篇
  2014年   2篇
  2013年   14篇
  2012年   9篇
  2011年   6篇
  2010年   8篇
  2009年   5篇
  2008年   4篇
  2007年   5篇
  2006年   8篇
  2005年   7篇
  2004年   7篇
  2003年   4篇
  2002年   6篇
  2001年   7篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   2篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   5篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   8篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   9篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   12篇
  1983年   5篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   2篇
  1979年   2篇
  1977年   3篇
  1975年   2篇
  1974年   5篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   2篇
  1970年   3篇
  1969年   4篇
  1966年   1篇
  1963年   1篇
  1961年   2篇
排序方式: 共有234条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
141.
142.
We investigate women’s and men’s willingness to engage in action on behalf of women, and we identify two distinct categories of behavior: action that aims to challenge gender inequality (feminist action) and action that aims to protect women from violence (protective action). Three online studies were conducted. For each study, a U.S. community sample was recruited. In Study 1 (n?=?602), women reported greater intentions to engage in feminist action than men did. Men, however, were just as willing as women to participate in protective action. In Study 2 (n?=?726), we replicated these gender differences and found that protective action was positively predicted by benevolent sexism among men. In Study 3 (N?=?582), we investigated why women reported greater intentions to engage in feminist action compared to men. We found that women were more aware of gender inequality, which was associated with identification as a feminist, and through this, intentions to engage in feminist action. Awareness of gender inequality also predicted intentions to engage in protective action among women. Men, however, were less aware of gender inequality, which was associated with the belief that feminist action leads to women having more rights than men do and subsequently greater willingness to participate in protective action. Our results can assist social policymakers and activists to develop appropriate campaigns for gender equality if their goal is to challenge, rather than protect women from, the status quo.  相似文献   
143.
The ability of a novice interviewer utilizing the ADIS to make diagnoses of PTSD in Vietnam veterans with combat experience was tested in a sample of 43 cases. Agreement between ADIS diagnosis and a criterion diagnosis was found in 93.0% of cases (κ = 0.857), suggesting that the diagnostic category can be reliably identified and demonstrating the utility of the ADIS for ascertaining this diagnosis.  相似文献   
144.
Book reviews     
Jacknedoff, R. (1985). Semantics and cognition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, Pp. 283. ISBN 0-262-60013-7. £ 9.95.

Lakoff, g. (1987). Women, fire and dangerous things : What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago, IL.: Chicago University Press. Pp. 624. ISBN 0-266-46803-8. £19.95.

Longuet-Higgins, H. C. (1987). Mental processes : Studies in cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Pp. 424. ISBN 0-262-12119-0. £22.50.

Nowakowska, M. (1986). Cognitive sciences: Basic problems, new perspectives, and implications for artificial intelligence. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Pp. 379. ISBN 0-12-522620-9. £44.50.

Winograd, T. & Flores, F. (1986). Understanding computers and cognition: A new foundation for design. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Pp. 207. ISBN 0-89391-050-3. £23.

Weisberg, R. W. (1986). Creativity: Genius and other myths. New York:W. H.Freeman and company. Pp. 169. ISBN 0-1767-1769-7. £9.95.

Bosshardt, H. R. S. G (1986). Perspektiven auf Sprache. Berlin : Walter de Gruyter. Pp. 326. ISBN 3-11-010068-1. DM134.00.  相似文献   
145.
146.
147.
148.
149.
150.
Although different types of prejudice tend to be highly correlated, target‐specific and more generalized components can nevertheless be distinguished. Here, we analyze whether indicators of the intergroup context—threat, contact, and neighborhood composition—predict the target‐specific and/or generalized components of prejudice. Using data from the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study (N = 4629), we build a multilevel model that captures the relationship between social dominance orientation, general levels of neighborhood heterogeneity, symbolic and realistic threat and cross‐group friendship (averaged across target groups), and generalized prejudice. Our model simultaneously estimates the relationship between target‐specific levels of these intergroup context indicators and target‐specific prejudice. Results indicated that social dominance orientation remained the strongest predictor of generalized prejudice when adjusting for other variables and that indicators of the intergroup context primarily explain differences between target group ratings. Aggregate levels of cross‐group friendship also had a small effect on generalized prejudice.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号