全文获取类型
收费全文 | 10400篇 |
免费 | 181篇 |
国内免费 | 31篇 |
专业分类
10612篇 |
出版年
2012年 | 1176篇 |
2011年 | 1324篇 |
2010年 | 293篇 |
2009年 | 124篇 |
2008年 | 977篇 |
2007年 | 1031篇 |
2006年 | 865篇 |
2005年 | 718篇 |
2004年 | 698篇 |
2003年 | 650篇 |
2002年 | 566篇 |
2001年 | 394篇 |
2000年 | 501篇 |
1999年 | 203篇 |
1998年 | 34篇 |
1997年 | 37篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 28篇 |
1993年 | 32篇 |
1992年 | 33篇 |
1991年 | 35篇 |
1990年 | 30篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 37篇 |
1987年 | 26篇 |
1986年 | 24篇 |
1985年 | 32篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 25篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 30篇 |
1980年 | 20篇 |
1979年 | 17篇 |
1978年 | 22篇 |
1976年 | 16篇 |
1965年 | 13篇 |
1963年 | 19篇 |
1959年 | 18篇 |
1958年 | 52篇 |
1957年 | 52篇 |
1956年 | 47篇 |
1955年 | 31篇 |
1954年 | 26篇 |
1953年 | 33篇 |
1952年 | 31篇 |
1951年 | 23篇 |
1950年 | 23篇 |
1949年 | 18篇 |
1948年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
211.
Goldberg C 《American journal of psychoanalysis》2002,62(2):185-199
Curiosity as a clinical entity has been a neglected subject in the psychoanalytic literature. Freud never addressed the issue of curiosity systematically. His interest was in trying to account for children's sexual questioning. Nevertheless, hinderance to internal curiosity—this is to say, that which intimidates and abates the appetite for an exploration of one's motives—is part and parcel of psychoanalytic inquiry. And, arguably, there is no greater clinical challenge for the analyst than trying to treat an analysand who appears to lack an interest in the underlying causes of his unhappiness. The problem of impeded self-inquiry is usually exacerbated in people with more serious emotional disturbances. My position here is that in studying the conditions that mitigate against curiosity in a seriously disturbed patient, we gain access to an enlarged version of the curiosity problems of our less disturbed patients. Here I interpret my clinical impressions about problems with curiosity with ideas from the writings of Martin Buber and Albert Camus. 相似文献
212.
Engelhard IM van den Hout MA Arntz A McNally RJ 《Behaviour research and therapy》2002,40(12):1415-1424
Previously, we found that chronic PTSD relates to “intrusion-based reasoning” (IR), i.e. the tendency to interpret distressing intrusions themselves as evidence that danger is impending, regardless of objective danger information (Engelhard et al., Behav. Res. Ther. 39 (2001) 1139). This study was intended to elucidate the causal status of this relation. Twenty-nine residents of a Belgian town witnessed a train crash and were assessed for IR and PTSD symptoms within 1 month and were re-assessed for PTSD at 3.5 months. Fourteen control residents did not witness the crash and were also tested for IR. The IR paradigm involved rating the danger of brief scenarios in which objective danger and presence of intrusions about the crash were systematically varied. The directly exposed residents showed greater danger ratings to scenarios in which intrusions were included than did the controls. IR was strongly related to both acute and chronic PTSD symptoms. It did not significantly predict chronic PTSD symptoms after controlling for acute symptoms, although the partial correlation (r=0.26, p=0.09) was in the expected direction. The data suggest that IR is involved in the onset and maintenance of PTSD symptoms, but more clarity about causality awaits future larger and experimental studies. 相似文献
213.
214.
Recognizing oneself, easy as it appears to be, seems at least to require awareness of one's body and one's actions. To investigate the contribution of these factors to self-recognition, we presented normal subjects with an image of both their own and the experimenter's hand. The hands could make the same, a different or no movement and could be displayed in various orientations. Subjects had to tell whether the indicated hand was theirs or not. The results showed that a congruence between visual signals and signals indicating the position of the body is one component on which self-recognition is based. Recognition of one's actions is another component. Subjects had most difficulty in recognizing their hand when movements were absent. When the two hands made different movements, subjects relied exclusively on the movement cue and recognition was almost perfect. Our findings are in line with pathological alterations in the sense of body and the sense of action. 相似文献
215.
Three experiments investigated whether the presence of more elements in the upper part of a configuration (i.e., up-down asymmetry) plays a role in determining newborns' preference for facelike patterns. Newborns preferred a nonfacelike stimulus with more elements in the upper part over a nonfacelike stimulus with more elements in the lower part (Experiment 1), did not show a preference for a facelike stimulus over a nonfacelike configuration equated for the number of elements in the upper part of the configuration (Experiment 2), and preferred a nonfacelike configuration located in the upper portion of the stimulus over a facelike configuration in the lower portion of the pattern (Experiment 3). Results demonstrated that up-down asymmetry is crucial in determining newborns' face preference. 相似文献
216.
Frankel B 《International journal of group psychotherapy》2002,52(2):215-231
Existential issues in group psychotherapy derive from existential thought both as a philosophy and as a value system. Its origins derive from the weakening of traditional values and the growing alienation of man from himself. The unique features of existentialism can be applied to all forms of therapy. These features are universal to humankind. They are finiteness, aloneness, guilt, responsibility, and freedom. In including existential concerns as part of group psychotherapy, therapist and patients move more closely to bilateral relationships and subjective interactions. 相似文献
217.
In group processes, acting-out has diverse functions, all of them equally important. It has an intrapsychic, interpersonal, and group dynamic function. Not only may it be understood as a form of resistance, but also in its communicative and reparative potential. The authors investigate the thesis that acting-out also contains the seed for change, thus helping patients divest themselves of pathological behavior. Using a group process as an example, this article shows how boundaries can be drawn between past and present experiences while using the communicative and reparative functions of acting-out. Unconscious psychodynamics can then be transformed from acting-out into dreams. 相似文献
218.
219.
Meta-analyses were conducted of 43 articles (with 48 different samples) investigating the relationship between parents' gender schemas and their offspring's gender-related cognitions. The parents' offspring ranged in age from infancy to early adulthood. Offspring measures included gender self-concept, gender attitudes toward others, gender-related interests, and occupational attitudes. Overall, a small but meaningful effect size (r = .16) indicated a significant and positive correlation between parent gender schemas and offspring measures. Specifically, parents with more traditional gender schemas were more likely than parents with more nontraditional schemas to have offspring with gender-typed cognitions about themselves or others. In addition, the magnitudes of observed effect sizes were influenced by particular moderator variables, including type of parent gender schema (gender self-concept vs. gender attitudes toward others), type of offspring gender-related cognitions, parent gender, offspring gender, offspring age, and publication characteristics. The results are cautiously interpreted as suggesting a possible influence of parents on the development of their children's gender-related thinking. 相似文献
220.
Age differences in coping resources and satisfaction with life among middle-aged,young-old,and oldest-old adults 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Hamarat E Thompson D Aysan F Steele D Matheny K Simons C 《The Journal of genetic psychology》2002,163(3):360-367
The authors examined age differences in perceived coping resources and satisfaction with life across 3 older-adult age groups (45-64, 65-74, and 75 years and older). The 98 participants represented healthy, socially active, community-residing adults. Group comparisons were made on 12 individual coping scales, and an overall coping resource effectiveness score was computed. No significant differences were found for 11 of the coping resources or for overall coping resource effectiveness. Similar consistencies in life satisfaction were found across the 3 age groups. The findings indicate that (a) for healthy adults, the oldest old cope at least as effectively as their younger counterparts, despite their likelihood of encountering increased levels of stress; and (b) psychologically, old age may be viewed as a time of resilience and fortitude. 相似文献