全文获取类型
收费全文 | 21895篇 |
免费 | 853篇 |
国内免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
22759篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 255篇 |
2018年 | 369篇 |
2017年 | 390篇 |
2016年 | 414篇 |
2015年 | 319篇 |
2014年 | 350篇 |
2013年 | 1748篇 |
2012年 | 627篇 |
2011年 | 647篇 |
2010年 | 386篇 |
2009年 | 365篇 |
2008年 | 496篇 |
2007年 | 548篇 |
2006年 | 473篇 |
2005年 | 448篇 |
2004年 | 422篇 |
2003年 | 412篇 |
2002年 | 443篇 |
2001年 | 747篇 |
2000年 | 710篇 |
1999年 | 537篇 |
1998年 | 215篇 |
1997年 | 199篇 |
1996年 | 222篇 |
1992年 | 450篇 |
1991年 | 441篇 |
1990年 | 427篇 |
1989年 | 400篇 |
1988年 | 419篇 |
1987年 | 377篇 |
1986年 | 388篇 |
1985年 | 386篇 |
1984年 | 308篇 |
1983年 | 276篇 |
1982年 | 207篇 |
1981年 | 205篇 |
1979年 | 349篇 |
1978年 | 253篇 |
1977年 | 214篇 |
1976年 | 199篇 |
1975年 | 279篇 |
1974年 | 355篇 |
1973年 | 367篇 |
1972年 | 288篇 |
1971年 | 284篇 |
1970年 | 281篇 |
1969年 | 251篇 |
1968年 | 349篇 |
1967年 | 307篇 |
1966年 | 284篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
42.
Wolfgang Hell Gerd Gigerenzer Siegfried Gauggel Maria Mall Michael Müller 《Memory & cognition》1988,16(6):533-538
If subjects are asked to recollect a former response after having been informed about the correct response, their recollection tends to approach the correct response. This effect has been termedhindsight bias. We studied hindsight bias in an experiment requiring numerical responses to almanac-type questions for physical quantities. We varied (1) the time at which the correct information was provided, (2) the encoding of the original responses by asking/not asking subjects to give a reason for the respective response, and (3) the motivation to recall correctly. We found that hindsight is less biased if reasons are given and if the correct information is provided at an earlier time. Motivation had only interactive effects: (1) With high motivation to recall correctly, the time the correct information was provided had no influence. (2) With reasons given, the variation of motivation showed no effect. These results rule out purely motivational and purely automatic explanations. 相似文献
43.
In most experiments in which the importance of visual control on postural stability is studied, optical stimuli attached to vertical surfaces are used. Analyses of long-term standing readjustments generally involve back-and-forth movements of a visual scene or its projection on vertical circular screens. In a natural environment, however, visual information is largely available from the ground. The aim of the experiment reported was to assess the effect of a flow pattern simulating an open outdoor setting on motion perception and postural control. Subjects were presented with an optical texture projected onto the ground. Periods of motionless texture alternated with equivalent durations of unidirectional flows. The change of position of the subject's centre of gravity over time was recorded on a force platform. Results show that the direction of body sway corresponded with that of texture motion. Important aftereffects, as shown in linear vection experiments, were also observed. However, the latency of postural responses was much shorter than with prolonged unidirectional flows produced in other locations of the visual environment. The hypothesis of an ecological specificity of the flows perceived on the ground during terrestrial displacements is discussed. 相似文献
44.
We studied the effect of different postoperative times on the behavioral recovery following brain implants. Adult male rats received cortical tissue grafts 2 weeks after aspiration of the medial frontal cortex. Either 2 (Immediate Group) or 28 (Delay Group) days after grafting, the performance of these rats on a behavioral battery, comprising the Morris water maze task, forepaw use, and grooming, was compared to that of rats with similar lesions and postoperative recovery times but no grafts. Rats tested immediately after receiving implants performed better on the spatial navigation task than rats with similar lesions but no grafts. This improvement, however, was less than that shown by rats with lesions but no grafts permitted to recover for 28 days before testing. In contrast, in the Delay Group, rats with grafts were more greatly impaired than were their operated controls. Neither lesions nor grafts affected grooming although the Immediate Group with grafts were significantly more impaired in using their forepaws during feeding than were any of the other groups. These results lead us to conclude that differing postoperative recovery times and task requirements may account for some of the inconsistent results of the influence of brain grafts on behavioral recovery reported in the literature. We also conclude that cortical tissue implants can have two effects with different time courses and opposite net behavioral effects. 相似文献
45.
46.
Past research in referential communication has indicated normally developing children show developmental progression in ability to communicate a specific referent to a listener. In one paradigm subjects were given lists of word-pairs in which one member of each pair was designated as the referent. It was shown that communicating about referents found in word-pairs associated in some way was more difficult than communicating about referents in dissimilar word-pairs. The present study extended this methodology to learning-disabled children. Learning-disabled, language-learning-disabled, and normally achieving children were asked to communicate about 30 pictured referents on three different tasks. On Tasks 1 and 2 each subject was asked to give a clue for the referent that would distinguish it from the other picture. Stimuli for Task 1 were 30 pairs of pictures that were related in some way and the stimuli for Task 2 were 30 pairs of unrelated pictures. Task 3 required the subjects to evaluate the adequacy of the examiner's clues for Task 1 stimuli. The disabled subjects were matched to the normally achieving subjects on the basis of receptive vocabulary age. Few differences were noted among the groups' performances on these referential communication tasks. Implications include the importance of vocabulary and concept development to referential communication. 相似文献
47.
Visual cognitive differences between hearing (N = 16) and deaf (N = 32) high-school and middle-school students were studied. Visual tasks were presented on a microcomputer and response latencies were collected. Significant differences were noted between the deaf and normal groups but not between total communication deaf and oral deaf students. These differences support the hypothesis that deaf students prefer a visual cognitive strategy. Implications for educating the deaf are discussed. 相似文献
48.
S W?sser W Handrick F B Spencker W Braun 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1988,40(2):95-101
Observations on three children with neurolisteriosis (one case of meningitis, two cases of meningoencephalitis, each Serovar 4 b), show that even after the neonatal period, listeriosis must not be ignored in the process of diagnosis and therapy. It is the bacteriological examination of the cerebrospinal fluid, together with the blood culture, and not clinical symptoms and serology that guarantee a timely diagnosis and therapy (ampicillin and gentamicin). 相似文献
49.
50.