全文获取类型
收费全文 | 52155篇 |
免费 | 1001篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
53161篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 483篇 |
2019年 | 656篇 |
2018年 | 887篇 |
2017年 | 956篇 |
2016年 | 956篇 |
2015年 | 725篇 |
2014年 | 802篇 |
2013年 | 4004篇 |
2012年 | 1411篇 |
2011年 | 1552篇 |
2010年 | 989篇 |
2009年 | 967篇 |
2008年 | 1330篇 |
2007年 | 1313篇 |
2006年 | 1185篇 |
2005年 | 1044篇 |
2004年 | 969篇 |
2003年 | 1005篇 |
2002年 | 978篇 |
2001年 | 1802篇 |
2000年 | 1752篇 |
1999年 | 1279篇 |
1998年 | 570篇 |
1997年 | 481篇 |
1996年 | 490篇 |
1992年 | 1080篇 |
1991年 | 1049篇 |
1990年 | 998篇 |
1989年 | 941篇 |
1988年 | 933篇 |
1987年 | 896篇 |
1986年 | 871篇 |
1985年 | 943篇 |
1984年 | 747篇 |
1983年 | 619篇 |
1981年 | 484篇 |
1979年 | 772篇 |
1978年 | 565篇 |
1977年 | 509篇 |
1976年 | 486篇 |
1975年 | 655篇 |
1974年 | 742篇 |
1973年 | 795篇 |
1972年 | 638篇 |
1971年 | 577篇 |
1970年 | 554篇 |
1969年 | 550篇 |
1968年 | 692篇 |
1967年 | 618篇 |
1966年 | 638篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
801.
Previous studies have identified three categories of variables which influence decisions of mock jurors: type of crime, defendant characteristics, and personal characteristics of jurors. This study manipulated the following variables towards the ends of assessing their influence on mock jurors' sentencing severity: premeditated vs. unpremeditated murder, black vs. white defendant, low SES vs. high SES defendant. Only defendant SES predicted sentencing severity: low SES defendants were assigned significantly longer sentences than high SES defendants. None of the measures of juror characteristics correlated with sentencing severity. Failure to replicate significant relationships with most of these variables and sentencing severity suggest that results of studies which manipulate only one variable dimension may overestimate the influence of these variable dimensions in mock jurors' decisions. 相似文献
802.
A A Leenaars W D Balance S Wenckstern D J Rudzinski 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》1985,15(3):184-195
This study of suicide notes involved the deduction of 50 protocol sentences that reflected important aspects of Shneidman's formulations regarding suicide. Independent judges noted the incidence of contents corresponding to the protocol sentences in 33 genuine and 33 simulated notes. Statements that were found significantly and more frequently in genuine notes included the following: the experience of adult trauma such as a calamitous relationship; a rejection; the withdrawal of a significant person resulting in despair; perceiving another as dooming one to the suicide; expressions of ambivalence; feelings of love, hate and other emotions; feelings of helplessness, pessimism and emotional confusion; appearing to be intoxicated by one's overpowering emotions and constricted perceptions; and the communications appearing to have unconscious implications. Statements of "egression" and more logical reasoning were more characteristic of the simulated notes. A frequency count in the genuine notes indicated that many protocol sentences were also very predictive of the content. Implications of the results are discussed. 相似文献
803.
804.
Willem A. Wagenaar Gideon B. Keren 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》1985,36(3):406-416
The question addressed in this study is whether experts are better calibrated than lay people. We investigated how well people are calibrated when they assess the probabilities of card combinations in the game of blackjack. Three groups of subjects were used: professional dealers, statistical experts, and control subjects. The results showed that experience and statistical expertise do not make people better calibrated in this task. It is argued that the concept of calibration is not wholly appropriate to describe the obtained deviations from the normatively correct responses. This is illustrated by a discriminant analysis performed on the signed deviation scores, which resulted in an almost perfect separation of the three groups, although they were overlapping with respect to calibration. 相似文献
805.
Betty B. Hoskins Ph.D. Helen Bequaert Holmes Ph.D. 《The Journal of medical humanities》1985,6(1):28-37
Life situations often seem to require dualistic, either or decision making, but this common method does not always clarify moral decisions. To show this, standard arguments on why to choose or not to choose the sex or ones child are presented. Then, our feminist thinking, which regards clusters of values, and which reframes questions rather than choosing between desirable alternatives, suggests another possibility, in a gynandrous world vision. 相似文献
806.
Timothy H. Monk Jeffrey E. Fookson Jacob Kream Margaret L. Moline Charles P. Pollak Muriel B. Weitzman 《Behavior research methods》1985,17(1):19-26
Cornell University Medical College, Westchester Division, The New York Hospital, White Plains, New York It is well established that there is a complex timekeeping mechanism in the human brain. This mechanism is associated with a variety of physiological and psychological rhythms having a period of about a day, and thus referred to as circadian rhythms. The circadian system has recently been modeled in terms of two underlying oscillators, one much more resistant to changes in routine than the other. These oscillators are considered to be endogenous, that is, internal to the organism, and not reliant for their existence upon changes in the person’s environment or general behavior. They thus continue to run even when the sleep/wake cycle is suspended, as in sustained operations. Thus, by their very nature, sustained operations require the individual to override the inputs that are coming from his or her circadian system (especially the indication that sleep is required). The aim of this paper is to provide a background to the area of circadian rhythms research, including a section on the methodology, so that the impact of the circadian system on sustained operations can be better understood. 相似文献
807.
In a dichotic listening situation stimuli were presented one at a time and at random to either ear of the subject at constant inter-stimulus intervals of 800 msec. The subject's task was to detect and count occasional slightly different stimuli in one ear. In Experiment 1, these ‘signal’ stimuli were slightly louder, and in Experiment 2 they had a slightly higher pitch, than the much more frequent, ‘standard’, stimuli. In both experiments signals occured randomly at either ear. Separate evoked potentials from three different locations were recorded for each of the four kinds of stimuli (attended signals, unattended signals, attended standards, unattended standards). Contrary to Hillyard et al. (1973), no early (N1 component) evoked-potential enhancement was observed to stimuli to the attended ear as compared with those to the unattended ear, but there was a later negative shift superimposed on potentials elicited by the former stimuli. This negative shift was considered identical to the N1 enhancement of Hillyard and his colleagues which in the present study was forced, by the longer inter-stimulus interval used, to demonstrate temporal dissociation with the N1 component. The ‘Hillyard effect’ was, consequently, explained as being caused by a superimposition of a CNV kind of negative shift on the evoked potential to the attended stimuli rather than by a growth of the ‘real’ N1 component of the evoked potential. 相似文献
808.
T.B. Rogers 《Acta psychologica》1978,42(1):21-28
The process of responding to attitude items was broken down into a series of cognitive stages and a model offered. To test this model, subjects responded to attitude items varying in Extremity under two or five response alternative formats. By measuring response times, and applying Sternberg's (1969) additive factor method, the model was supported. The results were discussed in terms of previous process work involving personality items and sentence verification tasks. 相似文献
809.
Bikkar S Randhawa 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1978,12(1):80-92
A report on the process and procedures of determining generic occupational skills and clusters of occupations for optimal occupational training needs is presented. Four hundred and ninety workers and their supervisors representing 37 different occupations were interviewed using a structured questionnaire consisting of items on mathematics, communication, reasoning, and interpersonal and manipulative skills. Respondents were asked to indicate whether a specific skill was a prerequisite to enter a job and also whether that specific skill was required on the job. Six, three, and two hierarchical skill factors and nine occupational clusters were identified. Training and curricular implications were explored. 相似文献
810.