首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   56036篇
  免费   2188篇
  国内免费   25篇
  58249篇
  2020年   588篇
  2019年   749篇
  2018年   1030篇
  2017年   1127篇
  2016年   1152篇
  2015年   829篇
  2014年   991篇
  2013年   4441篇
  2012年   1773篇
  2011年   1791篇
  2010年   1145篇
  2009年   1088篇
  2008年   1542篇
  2007年   1595篇
  2006年   1399篇
  2005年   1272篇
  2004年   1195篇
  2003年   1157篇
  2002年   1191篇
  2001年   1743篇
  2000年   1680篇
  1999年   1307篇
  1998年   568篇
  1997年   545篇
  1996年   538篇
  1995年   517篇
  1992年   1104篇
  1991年   1012篇
  1990年   1045篇
  1989年   939篇
  1988年   994篇
  1987年   911篇
  1986年   930篇
  1985年   867篇
  1984年   759篇
  1983年   709篇
  1982年   525篇
  1979年   856篇
  1978年   624篇
  1977年   531篇
  1975年   691篇
  1974年   805篇
  1973年   838篇
  1972年   654篇
  1971年   633篇
  1970年   590篇
  1969年   619篇
  1968年   768篇
  1967年   695篇
  1966年   609篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
32.
33.
The current study hypothesized that (1) hope would negatively predict burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, and acquired capability to enact lethal injury; (2) hope would negatively predict suicidal ideation; and (3) the interpersonal suicide risk factors would predict suicidal ideation. Results indicated that hope negatively predicted burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, but positively predicted acquired capability to enact suicide. Contrary to our second hypothesis, hope did not predict suicidal ideation, but interpersonal risk factors for suicide predicted suicidal ideation. Results are discussed in terms of implications for hope theory and Joiner's (2005) interpersonal risk factors for suicide, and for clinical practice.  相似文献   
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
The purpose of this paper is to study the convergent validity of the four most widely used burnout measures in a sample of Chinese nurses (N= 717). First, Structural Equation Modeling was used to investigate the factor structure of scores produced by the Maslach Burnout Inventory‐General Survey (MBI‐GS), the Burnout Measure (BM), the Shirom‐Melamed Burnout Measure (SMBM), and the Oldenburg Burnout Inventory (OLBI). Next, several competing models were tested to investigate the convergent validity of these four burnout instruments. The final results suggest that burnout is best conceived of as a multidimensional construct consisting of exhaustion and withdrawal, which are two related but conceptually distinct aspects. In addition, positively phrased items should be dropped from burnout measures for they constitute a separate factor that is considered to be an artifact.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号