全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4044篇 |
免费 | 102篇 |
国内免费 | 19篇 |
专业分类
4165篇 |
出版年
2012年 | 437篇 |
2011年 | 478篇 |
2010年 | 115篇 |
2009年 | 45篇 |
2008年 | 337篇 |
2007年 | 383篇 |
2006年 | 323篇 |
2005年 | 263篇 |
2004年 | 276篇 |
2003年 | 251篇 |
2002年 | 200篇 |
2001年 | 149篇 |
2000年 | 207篇 |
1999年 | 73篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 14篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 13篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 24篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 24篇 |
1987年 | 17篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 19篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1983年 | 15篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 17篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 8篇 |
1978年 | 17篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1965年 | 12篇 |
1963年 | 15篇 |
1959年 | 8篇 |
1958年 | 27篇 |
1957年 | 27篇 |
1956年 | 26篇 |
1955年 | 15篇 |
1954年 | 11篇 |
1953年 | 17篇 |
1952年 | 20篇 |
1951年 | 14篇 |
1950年 | 8篇 |
1949年 | 14篇 |
1948年 | 10篇 |
排序方式: 共有4165条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
12.
Interval and ratio reinforcement of a complex sequential operant in pigeons 总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0
下载免费PDF全文

Schwartz B 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1982,37(3):349-357
Pigeons were required to produce exactly four pecks on each of two keys in any order for reinforcement. Correct response sequences were reinforced on either fixed-interval two-minute or fixed-ratio four schedules, with each correct sequence treated as a single response. Each pigeon developed a particular dominant sequence that accounted for more than 80% of all sequences. Sequence stereotypy was relatively unaffected by the temporal properties of the fixed-interval and fixed-ratio schedules. Response time (time from the first response in each sequence to the last) was also relatively unaffected by the temporal properties of the schedules. In contrast, response latency (time from end of one sequence to the beginning of the next) was markedly affected by the schedules. Latencies were long early in the interreinforcement interval and got shorter as the interreinforcement interval progressed. These data suggest that stereotyped response sequences become functional behavioral units, resistant to disruption or alteration by reinforcement variables that ordinarily influence the temporal spacing of individual responses. 相似文献
13.
在苏联,由于经济和社会关系的伟大革命改造,建成了发达的社会主义社会。苏联的生活方式决定了社会意识的普遍无神论化,因此,在苏联社会生活中,科学无神论的社会功能和文化功能得以充实和扩大,科学无神论同物质和精神实践的各方面、各领域的联系得以巩固和完善。在信教者中间,虔诚狂热的信徒逐渐减少,这类现象证明,宗教在我国社会中的影响正在减退。 相似文献
14.
In this article, the authors present two laboratory experiments testing a group-level perspective on the role of empathy in helping. Experiment 1 tested the authors' predictions in an intercultural context of helping. Confirming their specific Empathy x Group Membership moderation hypothesis, empathy had a stronger effect on helping intentions when the helper and the target belonged to the same cultural group than when they belonged to different groups. Experiment 2 replicated these findings in a modified minimal group paradigm using laboratory-created groups. Moreover, this second experiment also provides evidence for the hypothesized psychological mechanisms underlying the empathy-(ingroup) helping relationship. Specifically, analyses in the ingroup condition confirmed that the strength of the empathy-(ingroup) helping relationship systematically varied as a function of perceived similarities among ingroup members. The general implications of these findings for empathy-motivated helping are discussed. 相似文献
15.
De Dreu CK Beersma B Stroebe K Euwema MC 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2006,90(6):927-943
The authors tested a motivated information-processing model of negotiation: To reach high joint outcomes, negotiators need a deep understanding of the task, which requires them to exchange information and to process new information systematically. All this depends on social motivation, epistemic motivation (EM), and their interaction. Indeed, when EM (manipulated by holding negotiators process accountability or not) was high rather than low and prosocial rather than proself, negotiators recall more cooperative than competitive tactics (Experiment 1), had more trust, and reached higher joint outcomes (Experiment 2). Experiment 3 showed that under high EM, negotiators who received cooperative, rather than competitive, tactics reached higher joint outcomes because they engaged in more problem solving. Under low EM, negotiators made more concessions and reached low joint outcomes. Implications for negotiation theory and for future work in this area are discussed. 相似文献
16.
Hartsuiker RJ Antón-Méndez I Roelstraete B Costa A 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2006,32(4):949-953
Lexical bias is the tendency for phonological errors to form existing words at a rate above chance. This effect has been observed in experiments and corpus analyses in Germanic languages, but S. del Viso, J. M. Igoa, and J. E. García-Albea (1991) found no effect in a Spanish corpus study. Because lexical bias plays an important role in the debate on interactivity in language production, the authors reconsidered its absence in Spanish. A corpus analysis, which considered relatively many errors and which used a method of estimating chance rate that is relatively independent of total error number, and a speech-error elicitation experiment provided converging evidence for lexical bias in Spanish. The authors conclude that the processing mechanisms underlying this effect hold cross-linguistically. 相似文献
17.
This article introduces two new indirect measures of anxiety that are based on the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task (EAST; De Houwer, 2003). The EAST differs from the more established Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) in that participants' responses to different trials within one block of trials are compared rather than performance between two different blocks of trials. Two studies led to the following results: (a) Both extrinsic Simon tasks for assessing anxiety showed only moderate internal consistencies, (b) one of the two tasks showed at least some convergent validity with an IAT for assessing anxiety, and (c) both tasks were dissociated from self-reported anxiety. Implications for future adaptations of the EAST for the indirect assessment of personality dimensions are discussed. 相似文献
18.
The main determinants of lexical access in speech are considered to be a word's age of acquisition (AoA) and its frequency of occurrence in a speaker's experience. It is unclear whether and how these variables interact, although they are commonly observed to be correlated, for the few studies that address the issue have reported inconsistent findings. An influential view of AoA in lexical processing (Ellis and Lambon Ralph, 2000) predicts stronger frequency effects for items acquired later in life than for those acquired at an early age. Five experiments were designed to investigate the possible interaction of AoA and frequency effects in speech. We found that the interaction between word frequency and AoA was not robust and that, contrary to expectation, the effect of word frequency was greater for words acquired earlier in life than for those acquired later. The implications of our findings are discussed. 相似文献
19.
20.
In these studies, the correlates of spontaneously using expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal during stressful speeches were examined. Spontaneous emotion regulation means that there were no instructions of how to regulate emotions during the speech. Instead, participants indicated after the speech to what extent they used self-motivated expressive suppression or reappraisal during the task. The results show that suppression is associated with less anxiety expression, greater physiological responding, and less memory for the speech while having no impact on negative affect. In contrast, reappraisal has no impact on physiology and memory while leading to less expression and affect. Taken together, spontaneous emotion regulation in active coping tasks has similar consequences as experimentally induced emotion regulation in passive tasks. 相似文献