全文获取类型
收费全文 | 12834篇 |
免费 | 588篇 |
国内免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
13426篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 163篇 |
2019年 | 164篇 |
2018年 | 287篇 |
2017年 | 262篇 |
2016年 | 271篇 |
2015年 | 197篇 |
2014年 | 228篇 |
2013年 | 1045篇 |
2012年 | 381篇 |
2011年 | 425篇 |
2010年 | 265篇 |
2009年 | 230篇 |
2008年 | 356篇 |
2007年 | 346篇 |
2006年 | 317篇 |
2005年 | 277篇 |
2004年 | 263篇 |
2003年 | 239篇 |
2002年 | 242篇 |
2001年 | 450篇 |
2000年 | 430篇 |
1999年 | 306篇 |
1998年 | 140篇 |
1997年 | 112篇 |
1996年 | 115篇 |
1995年 | 114篇 |
1992年 | 243篇 |
1991年 | 244篇 |
1990年 | 219篇 |
1989年 | 216篇 |
1988年 | 219篇 |
1987年 | 211篇 |
1986年 | 195篇 |
1985年 | 189篇 |
1984年 | 184篇 |
1983年 | 145篇 |
1982年 | 147篇 |
1981年 | 114篇 |
1979年 | 178篇 |
1977年 | 116篇 |
1975年 | 159篇 |
1974年 | 185篇 |
1973年 | 207篇 |
1972年 | 176篇 |
1971年 | 161篇 |
1970年 | 143篇 |
1969年 | 149篇 |
1968年 | 191篇 |
1967年 | 148篇 |
1966年 | 151篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
In three experiments, we examined the mechanisms by which prior experience with proactive interference (PI) diminished its
effects. Cued recall tasks conforming to an A–B, A–D paradigm were used to induce PI effects. Experiment 1 showed that reduced
PI was not due to a reduction in attention to the source of PI. Experiment 2 revealed that participants’ awareness of PI effects
on memory performance increased with experience, resulting in a shift in encoding processes. Experiment 3 demonstrated that
changes in encoding provided additional support for recollection that further enhanced participants’ ability to constrain
their retrieval processing to the appropriate source of information at the time of test. These results can be interpreted
as showing that experience with PI enhances awareness of its effects and allows individuals to adjust their learning and retrieval
strategies to compensate for such effects. 相似文献
992.
Although the measurement of working memory capacity is crucial to understanding working memory and its interaction with other cognitive faculties, there are inconsistencies in the literature on how to measure capacity. We address the measurement in the change detection paradigm, popularized by Luck and Vogel (Nature, 390, 279-281, 1997). Two measures for this task-from Pashler (Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 369-378, 1988) and Cowan (The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87-114, 2001), respectively-have been used interchangeably, even though they may yield qualitatively different conclusions. We show that the choice between these two measures is not arbitrary. Although they are motivated by the same underlying discrete-slots working memory model, each is applicable only to a specific task; the two are never interchangeable. In the course of deriving these measures, we discuss subtle but consequential flaws in the underlying discrete-slots model. These flaws motivate revision in the modal model and capacity measures. 相似文献
993.
Odell S Sander E Denson LA Baldassano RN Hommel KA 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2011,18(1):39-45
The objective of this study was to examine the relative contributions of both parental and adolescent functioning to family functioning in adolescent patients with inflammatory bowel disease (IBD) and their families. Participants were 45 adolescents (27 male, 18 female) 13-17?years old (M?=?15.41?years, SD?=?1.32) with IBD and their parents. Families completed measures of patient behavioral functioning and depression, parent distress and family functioning. Disease severity assessments were completed via data provided by patients' gastroenterologists. Results indicated that parent-reported patient behavioral problems accounted for a significant 26% of variance in family functioning. Post-hoc analysis revealed that externalizing behavior problems accounted for the majority of this variance compared to internalizing behavior problems. These results suggest that externalizing problems may have a more significant impact on these families than previous research indicates. Moreover, externalizing behaviors may significantly impact family adaptation and should be taken into consideration during routine clinical care. Further research is needed to replicate and expand upon these findings. 相似文献
994.
Marques L LeBlanc N Weingarden H Greenberg JL Traeger LN Keshaviah A Wilhelm S 《Body image》2011,8(2):163-167
Differences in the presentation of clinical features of body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) across ethnic groups have received little investigation. The current study assessed BDD symptoms in an ethnically diverse sample of adults (n=401) using an online survey. Participants completed self-report measures assessing BDD symptoms, body parts of concern and BDD behaviors. Compared to Caucasian participants, no significant differences were found in body parts or behaviors reported by Latino or African American participants. Significant group differences did emerge between Asian and Caucasian participants. Specifically, Asians reported more concern with straight hair and dark skin and fewer body shape concerns than Caucasians. Asians also endorsed lower rates of grooming, touching body parts, and camouflaging and higher rates of exercise compared to Caucasians. Although most clinical features of BDD appear similar across ethnic groups, results showed some differences in body parts and behaviors between Caucasians and Asian Americans with BDD symptoms. 相似文献
995.
996.
997.
This study explored the current psychological characteristics and criminal behavior history of individuals who retrospectively reported being bullies, bully-victims, victims, or controls (i.e. neither victims nor bullies) during their last 2 years of high school. College students (n = 960) completed measures of criminal thinking, aggression, psychopathy, and criminal behavior online. We predicted bullies and bully-victims would demonstrate the highest scores for criminal thinking, proactive aggression, psychopathy, and have the most criminal infractions. Bullies and bully-victims had significantly higher scores on criminal thinking, aggression, psychopathy, and criminal behaviors than victims or controls. Additionally, men were significantly higher in criminal thinking, aggression, psychopathy, and had more criminal acts than women. There were no gender by bully group interactions. Logistic regression analyses differentiated bully-victims from bullies. Bully-victims tended to be male, higher in criminal thinking, and higher in reactive aggression. In addition, bully-victims were distinct from victims, showing higher criminal thinking and higher proactive aggression. 相似文献
998.
Most research on sexual harassment has involved undergraduate students and European Americans, whose perspectives may not be representative of the broader population. This study investigated whether judgments of a sexual harassment trial vary by plaintiff ethnicity (European American or Latin American), type of sample (undergraduates or community members) and mock juror ethnicity (European American or Latin American). We also tested the effects of a cultural relativist argument about Latin American cultural values influencing the plaintiff. Results indicated that community and Latin American mock jurors rendered more pro-plaintiff verdicts, particularly when the case did not include a cultural relativist argument. Although the cultural relativist argument did not affect undergraduates' judgments, it caused a backlash among community members, leading to more pro-defendant verdicts. Judgments across type of sample and mock juror ethnicity were partially mediated by self-referencing and hostile sexism; affiliation with Latin American culture also predicted judgments of the Latina American plaintiff. 相似文献
999.
The current study shows that spatial visual attention is used to retrieve information from visual working memory. Participants had to keep four colored circles in visual working memory. While keeping this information in memory we asked whether one of the colors was present in the array. While retrieving this information, on some trials a probe dot was presented. When this probe dot was presented at the location of the color that had to be retrieved, participants responded faster than when it was presented at another location. Our findings further elaborate the role of visual attention in working memory: not only is attention the mechanism by which information is stored into working memory, it is also the mechanism by which information is retrieved from visual working memory. 相似文献
1000.
Accurate timing performance during auditory–motor synchronization has been well documented for finger tapping tasks. It is believed that information pertaining to an event in movement production aids in detecting and correcting for errors between movement cycle completion and the metronome tone. Tasks with minimal event-related information exhibit more variable synchronization and less rapid error correction. Recent work from our laboratory has indicated that a task purportedly lacking an event structure (circle drawing) did not exhibit accurate synchronization or error correction (Studenka & Zelaznik, in press). In the present paper we report on two experiments examining synchronization in tapping and circle drawing tasks. In Experiment 1, error correction processes of an event-timed tapping timing task and an emergently timed circle drawing timing task were examined. Rapid and complete error correction was seen for the tapping, but not for the circle drawing task. In Experiment 2, a perceptual event was added to delineate a cycle in circle drawing, and the perceptual event of table contact was removed from the tapping task. The inclusion of an event produced a marked improvement in synchronization error correction for circle drawing, and the removal of tactile feedback (taking away an event) slightly reduced the error correction response of tapping. Furthermore, the task kinematics of circle drawing remained smooth providing evidence that event structure can be kinematic or perceptual in nature. Thus, synchronization and error correction, characteristic of event timing (Ivry, Spencer, Zelaznik, & Diedrichsen, 2002; Repp, 2005), depends upon the presence of a distinguishable source of sensory information at the timing goal. 相似文献