全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1321篇 |
免费 | 22篇 |
专业分类
1343篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 13篇 |
2018年 | 18篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 20篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 21篇 |
2013年 | 185篇 |
2012年 | 29篇 |
2011年 | 31篇 |
2010年 | 24篇 |
2009年 | 33篇 |
2008年 | 44篇 |
2007年 | 53篇 |
2006年 | 42篇 |
2005年 | 45篇 |
2004年 | 46篇 |
2003年 | 40篇 |
2002年 | 36篇 |
2001年 | 26篇 |
2000年 | 23篇 |
1999年 | 21篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 17篇 |
1994年 | 15篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 28篇 |
1991年 | 21篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 16篇 |
1982年 | 24篇 |
1981年 | 26篇 |
1980年 | 13篇 |
1979年 | 18篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1977年 | 13篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1975年 | 16篇 |
1974年 | 19篇 |
1973年 | 20篇 |
1972年 | 13篇 |
1971年 | 14篇 |
1970年 | 13篇 |
1969年 | 13篇 |
排序方式: 共有1343条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
92.
93.
Baldo Juliana V. Shimamura Arthur P. Prinzmetal William 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1998,60(3):427-437
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics - Response compatibility effects were assessed with a Stroop-like task which involved arrow and word stimuli. The subjects were required to respond to one... 相似文献
94.
95.
96.
J. Wesley Libb Srdjan Stankovic Roberta Sokol Arthur Freeman Carl Houck Paul Switzer 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(1-2):209-218
The Milton Clinical Multiaxial Inventory (MCMI) is a promising, yet somewhat unproven psychometric inventory developed to identify clinical syndromes and personality traits consonant with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed. [DSM-III]; American Psychiatric Association, 1980). The stability of its measures for both the theoretically more stable personality characteristics and the clinical syndromes was investigated in a group of depressed psychiatric outpatients. In this test-retest design with a 3-month interval between tests, clinical syndrome scales of relevance changed significantly as expected. However, many of the personality scales also changed significantly. Only four of the personality scales met a two-fold test of stability. Findings are discussed in terms of characteristics of self-report inventories such as the MCMI, the uniqueness of the depressed population, and characteristics of personality disorders. 相似文献
97.
Donald Edmondson Jonathan A. Shaffer William F. Chaplin Matthew M. Burg Arthur A. Stone Joseph E. Schwartz 《Journal of research in personality》2013,47(6):843-852
Ecological momentary assessments (EMA) of anxiety and anger/hostility were obtained every 25–30 min over two 24-h periods, separated by a median of 6 months, from 165 employees at a university in the Northeast. We used a multilevel trait-state-error structural equation model to estimate: (1) the proportion of variance in EMA anxiety and anger/hostility attributable to stable trait-like individual differences; (2) the correspondence between these trait-like components of EMA anxiety and anger/hostility and traditional questionnaire measures of each construct; and (3) the test–retest correlation between two 24-h averages obtained several months apart. After adjustment for measurement error, more than half the total variance in EMA reports of anxiety and anger/hostility is attributable to stable trait-like individual differences; however, the trait-like component of each construct is only modestly correlated with questionnaire measures of that construct. The 6-month “test–retest” correlations of latent variables representing the true 24-h EMA average anxiety and average anger are quite high (r ? 0.83). This study represents the longest follow-up period over which EMA-based estimates of traits have been examined. The results suggest that although the trait component (individual differences) of EMA momentary ratings of anxiety and anger is larger than the state component, traditional self-report questionnaires of trait anxiety and anger correspond only weakly with EMA-defined traits. 相似文献
98.
Yusuke Yamani Jason S. McCarley Jeffrey R. W. Mounts Arthur F. Kramer 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2013,75(2):229-243
A pair of experiments investigated the architecture of visual processing, parallel versus serial, across high and low levels of spatial interference in a divided attention task. Subjects made speeded judgments that required them to attend to a pair of color-cued objects among gray filler items, with the spatial proximity between the attended items varied to manipulate the strength of interference between attended items. Systems factorial analysis (Townsend & Nozawa, Journal of Mathematical Psychology 39:321-359, 1995) was used to identify processing architecture. Experiment 1, using moderately dense displays, found evidence of parallel processing whether attended objects were in low or high proximity to one another. Experiment 2, using higher-density displays, found evidence of parallel selection when attended stimuli were widely separated but serial processing when they were in high proximity. Divided visual attention can operate in parallel under conditions of low or moderate spatial interference between selected items, but strong interference engenders serial processing. 相似文献
99.
Attentional selection of an object in the visual field degrades processing of neighboring stimuli in young adults. A pair of experiments examined the effects of aging on such localized attentional interference. In Experiment 1, younger and older observers made speeded same-different judgments of target shapes that varied in spatial separation. Performance declined for both age groups as the distance between targets decreased, but an Age x Distance interaction indicated that the magnitude of this effect was larger for older adults. Experiment 2 ruled out sensory masking as an explanation for these findings. Results indicate that older observers experience losses in the ability to attend to multiple spatially proximal stimuli within the visual field. 相似文献
100.
This study examined the relationship between the similarity and accuracy of team mental models and compared the extent to which each predicted team performance. The relationship between team ability composition and team mental models was also investigated. Eighty-three dyadic teams worked on a complex skill task in a 2-week training protocol. Results indicated that although similarity and accuracy of team mental models were significantly related, accuracy was a stronger predictor of team performance. In addition, team ability was more strongly related to the accuracy than to the similarity of team mental models and accuracy partially mediated the relationship between team ability and team performance, but similarity did not. 相似文献