首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   951篇
  免费   36篇
  2020年   15篇
  2019年   13篇
  2018年   18篇
  2017年   22篇
  2016年   22篇
  2015年   17篇
  2014年   18篇
  2013年   123篇
  2012年   34篇
  2011年   35篇
  2010年   19篇
  2009年   19篇
  2008年   38篇
  2007年   32篇
  2006年   23篇
  2005年   28篇
  2004年   22篇
  2003年   27篇
  2002年   23篇
  2001年   12篇
  2000年   18篇
  1999年   11篇
  1997年   13篇
  1995年   10篇
  1994年   9篇
  1993年   16篇
  1992年   12篇
  1991年   9篇
  1990年   16篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   16篇
  1987年   7篇
  1986年   11篇
  1985年   15篇
  1984年   12篇
  1983年   15篇
  1982年   9篇
  1981年   9篇
  1980年   14篇
  1979年   13篇
  1978年   21篇
  1977年   15篇
  1976年   11篇
  1975年   12篇
  1974年   11篇
  1973年   14篇
  1972年   9篇
  1971年   7篇
  1968年   11篇
  1967年   10篇
排序方式: 共有987条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
971.
972.
973.
974.
Abstract

A poor understanding of probability may lead people to misinterpret every day coincidences and form anomalistic (e.g., paranormal) beliefs. We investigated the relationship between anomalistic belief (including type of belief) and misperception of chance and the base rate fallacy across both anomalistic and control (i.e., neutral) contexts. Greater anomalistic belief was associated with poorer performance for both types of items; however there were no significant interactions between belief and context. For misperception of chance items, only experiential (vs. theoretical) anomalistic beliefs predicted more errors. In contrast, overall anomalistic belief was positively related to the base rate fallacy but no specific subtype of anomalistic belief was a significant predictor. The results indicate misperception of chance may lead people to interpret coincidental events as having an anomalistic cause, and a poor understanding of base rates may make people more prone to forming anomalistic beliefs.  相似文献   
975.
Organizations frequently screen or select job applicants based on their work experience. Despite this, surprisingly little is known about the criterion-related validity of prehire experience, which reflects the amount, duration, or type of experience workers have acquired before they enter a new organization. To address this critical gap in the literature, we used meta-analysis to synthesize data from 81 independent samples that reported relations between prehire experience and performance or turnover. Results revealed overall corrected correlations of .06 for job performance (k = 44, n = 11,785), .11 for training performance (k = 21, n = 8,176), and .00 for turnover (k = 32, n = 11,676). Measures that capture prehire experience with tasks, jobs, or occupations relevant to workers’ current position also are only weakly related to the outcomes (e.g.,  = .07 for job performance). Two exceptions to our main findings are that (a) prehire experience is somewhat more predictive of job performance when workers first start a new job and (b) measures of task-level experience predict training performance, although these results are based on small subsets of primary studies. Overall, the present findings suggest that the types of prehire experience measures organizations currently use to screen job applicants generally are poor predictors of future performance and turnover. We therefore caution organizations from selecting employees based on such measures unless more positive evidence emerges.  相似文献   
976.
977.
978.
979.
980.
The classical urn scheme is further extended to permit application to simple learning situations with arbitrary response and reinforcement spaces. The assumptions incorporated are those which, in the classical situation, are equivalent to (1) permitting the total number of balls added per trial to depend on the preceding reinforcement and (2) allowing the probability that a ball is drawn to depend on the time the ball has been in the urn. This results in a flexible family of models whose behavior is analyzed in the case of noncontingent reinforcement.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号