首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   3505篇
  免费   170篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2023年   31篇
  2022年   41篇
  2021年   48篇
  2020年   93篇
  2019年   88篇
  2018年   142篇
  2017年   148篇
  2016年   123篇
  2015年   106篇
  2014年   93篇
  2013年   373篇
  2012年   152篇
  2011年   157篇
  2010年   90篇
  2009年   82篇
  2008年   122篇
  2007年   129篇
  2006年   108篇
  2005年   100篇
  2004年   98篇
  2003年   77篇
  2002年   69篇
  2001年   139篇
  2000年   135篇
  1999年   88篇
  1998年   22篇
  1997年   35篇
  1996年   30篇
  1995年   19篇
  1994年   26篇
  1993年   18篇
  1992年   46篇
  1991年   57篇
  1990年   50篇
  1989年   47篇
  1988年   44篇
  1987年   32篇
  1986年   19篇
  1985年   23篇
  1984年   26篇
  1983年   19篇
  1979年   24篇
  1977年   18篇
  1975年   17篇
  1974年   20篇
  1973年   17篇
  1971年   20篇
  1969年   18篇
  1968年   18篇
  1967年   24篇
排序方式: 共有3676条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
911.
The purposes of this study were to evaluate the effects of self-monitoring on increasing the on-task behaviors of four at-risk middle school students, and to examine its indirect effects on academic performance. Additionally, we attempted to develop and validate cost- and time-efficient procedures that typical classroom teachers could use. The data from a multiple baseline design across three academic settings, replicated with four students, revealed immediate increases in on-task behavior as each student began to self-monitor in each sequential setting. The data also revealed slightly higher levels of academic performance and, in most instances, gradually accelerating trends. These findings suggest that self-monitoring is a cost- and time-efficient procedure that classroom teachers can use to teach at-risk students to manage their own behaviors.  相似文献   
912.
Book Review     
Miklósi  Ádám 《Animal cognition》1998,1(2):135-135
  相似文献   
913.
914.
The main goal of the present study was to determine whether observation of an unskilled model learning a timing task enables the observer to develop a cognitive representation of the task similar to the one acquired through physical practice (Adams, 1986; Bandura, 1977; Lee & White, 1990). To reach that goal, we tested whether a contextual interference effect would be obtained in a retention test of subjects who had observed an individual practicing three variations of a timing task under a random or a blocked schedule of practice. Similar patterns of results in an immediate retention test were found following observation and physical practice. This suggests that observation indeed engaged the observers in the same type of cognitive activities as did physical practice. Moreover, a schedule of practice made up of 100% physical practice led to improved learning compared with a schedule of practice made up of 50% observation followed by 50% physical practice. This suggests that learning is enhanced more by numerous implementations of a motor program than by its mere construction or retrieval.  相似文献   
915.
Summary We recall how Hasbroucq and Guiard (1991) defined stimulus (S) congruity, the SS-correspondence factor they demonstrated to account for both the Simon effect and the Hedge and Marsh effect, and how they explained the mechanisms that bring S congruity into play in the verbal and nonverbal versions of the Simon task and in the Hedge and Marsh task. We show that, contrary to O'Leary, Barber, and Simon's claim, Hasbroucq and Guiard used a consistent definition of S congruity across the various tasks of interest. Finally, we recognize that Hasbroucq and Guiard's experiments produced no direct falsification of the display control arrangement correspondence (DCC) hypothesis — the special-purpose hypothesis put forward by Simon and his group as an attempt to reconcile the Hedge and Marsh effect with their interpretation of the Simon effect in terms of irrelevant spatial SR correspondence —, but we argue that the DCC account is unnecessary and implausible.  相似文献   
916.
The WPPSI, WISC-R, and WPPSI-R were administered in a counterbalanced design to 72 children between the ages of 5 years, 10 months, 16 days, and 6 years, 7 months, and 15 days to test the hypothesis that mean verbal IQs on these scales would be higher for boys than for girls. Results indicated that the mean verbal IQs for boys were significantly (p<.05) higher than those for girls on all three scales. Also, significant (p<.05) differences favoring boys were found onVocabulary andComprehension subtests on all three scales. These differences are probably a peculiarity of these scales and may not need any elaborate theoretical explanation. However, clinical uses of verbal and performance IQ discrepancies may not justifiably ignore the examinees’ gender.  相似文献   
917.
The syntactic characterization of agrammatism   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Y Grodzinsky 《Cognition》1984,16(2):99-120
  相似文献   
918.
919.
This study investigated how sensorimotor activities interact with the geometric characteristics and the meaning of an object during development of writing. These changes were studied in 4- to 7-yr.-old girls. Analysis showed that visuo-spatial activities play a role in denomination and graphic activities, especially during critical periods of development. These activities have varying effects at different stages of development. The spatial factors studied here act differently in a verbal task (denomination) and in a perceptual-motor activity (graphic activity).  相似文献   
920.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号