首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   6085篇
  免费   89篇
  国内免费   1篇
  6175篇
  2019年   57篇
  2018年   110篇
  2017年   89篇
  2016年   83篇
  2015年   65篇
  2014年   75篇
  2013年   452篇
  2012年   176篇
  2011年   165篇
  2010年   90篇
  2009年   100篇
  2008年   139篇
  2007年   156篇
  2006年   132篇
  2005年   127篇
  2004年   123篇
  2003年   104篇
  2002年   117篇
  2001年   214篇
  2000年   206篇
  1999年   142篇
  1998年   56篇
  1996年   59篇
  1993年   54篇
  1992年   118篇
  1991年   121篇
  1990年   117篇
  1989年   99篇
  1988年   89篇
  1987年   97篇
  1986年   108篇
  1985年   105篇
  1984年   95篇
  1983年   82篇
  1981年   54篇
  1979年   95篇
  1978年   70篇
  1977年   62篇
  1976年   54篇
  1975年   67篇
  1974年   92篇
  1973年   100篇
  1972年   72篇
  1971年   70篇
  1970年   80篇
  1969年   75篇
  1968年   106篇
  1967年   82篇
  1966年   93篇
  1958年   62篇
排序方式: 共有6175条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
911.
912.
    
  相似文献   
913.
  总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
We studied three groups of children (mute autistics, autistics with echolalic speech, and normals) under two conditions (while the children were engaged in self-stimulation vs. free from self-stimulatory behavior). The children were trained to approach a dispenser, for candy reinforcement, at the sound of a tone. The time interval between the onset of the tone and Ss approach was labelled his response latency. We found that (1) the presence of self-stimulatory behavior was associated with increased response latencies for the mute autistics; (2) the response latencies of the latter group decreased as Ss received increased training in responding to the auditory stimulus; (3) the amount of self-stimulatory behavior varied with the magnitude of reinforcement for other behavior; (4) in a preliminary way, we obtained some control over the response latencies by experimentally manipulating the amount of self-stimulatory behavior. The data were conceptualized in terms of competing reinforcers.  相似文献   
914.
    
Male rats were either handled daily or left undisturbed (nonhandled) during the first 21 days of life. At weaning (Day 22) animals in each early treatment condition were assigned to two housing conditions, isolated or grouped, creating four early/late treatment conditions: Handled-Grouped (HG), Handled-Isolated (HI), Nonhandled-Grouped (NHG), and Nonhandled-Isolated (NHI). At maturity, all animals were tested in the latent inhibition (LI, Experiment 1) and the partial reinforcement extinction effect (PREE, Experiment 2) paradigms. In LI, animals receiving prior nonreinforced preexposure to a stimulus (PE) show subsequently retarded conditioning to that stimulus as compared to nonpreexposed (NPE) controls. In the PREE, partially reinforced (PRF) animals exhibit higher resistance to extinction as compared to continuously reinforced (CRF) controls. LI and PREE were obtained in the HG condition, but were impaired in NHG and HI animals. The pattern of impairment was different in the two conditions. The absence of LI in NHG males was due to increased suppression in the PE group, whereas in HI males, was due to decreased suppression in the NPE group. Likewise, the attenuation of the PREE in NHG males stemmed primarily from decreased resistance to extinction in the PRF group, whereas in HI males, it resulted from increased resistance to extinction in the CRF group. The combination of nonhandling and isolation led to the reinstatement of both LI and PREE: normal LI and PREE were obtained in the NHI animals. These results demonstrate that early rearing experience interacts with later experience differentially depending upon the later experience.  相似文献   
915.
    
It has generally been assumed that increases in the concrete outcomes of a procedure will result in judgments of greater procedural and distributive fairness, but research on this topic has been inconsistent. Using a classic procedural justice paradigm (Walker, LaTour, Lind, &. Thibaut, 1974), the experiment tested the effects of four levels of outcome. Forty-eight male and female undergraduates were led to believe that their team had been wrongfully charged with cheating in a business simulation game. An adversary adjudication was held, purportedly to resolve the charge. The outcome of the adjudication was confiscation of all, two thirds, one third, or none of the subject's monetary winnings from the game. Both procedural and distributive fairness measures showed dear nonlinear outcome effects. The relationship between outcomes and both fairness measures showed some evidence of being nonmonotonic as well: A two-thirds loss resulted in less favorable reactions than did a total loss. Ratings on other scales suggest that the nonlinear effects are due to beliefs that the judge did not fully consider the evidence in the two-thirds loss and one-third loss conditions. The discussion focuses on the theoretical implications of the findings for procedural justice and social exchange processes and the practical implications for conflict resolution procedure.  相似文献   
916.
  总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
917.
918.
919.
    
  相似文献   
920.
    
E Taub  I A Goldberg 《Perception》1974,3(4):393-405
  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号