全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2013篇 |
免费 | 23篇 |
专业分类
2036篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 13篇 |
2023年 | 15篇 |
2022年 | 12篇 |
2021年 | 29篇 |
2020年 | 28篇 |
2019年 | 39篇 |
2018年 | 48篇 |
2017年 | 56篇 |
2016年 | 71篇 |
2015年 | 51篇 |
2014年 | 47篇 |
2013年 | 210篇 |
2012年 | 103篇 |
2011年 | 93篇 |
2010年 | 62篇 |
2009年 | 60篇 |
2008年 | 77篇 |
2007年 | 88篇 |
2006年 | 63篇 |
2005年 | 65篇 |
2004年 | 67篇 |
2003年 | 60篇 |
2002年 | 73篇 |
2001年 | 28篇 |
2000年 | 27篇 |
1999年 | 28篇 |
1998年 | 37篇 |
1997年 | 29篇 |
1996年 | 26篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 26篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 21篇 |
1991年 | 23篇 |
1990年 | 16篇 |
1989年 | 20篇 |
1988年 | 20篇 |
1987年 | 14篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 21篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 14篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 15篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 13篇 |
1973年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有2036条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
CONCEPT LEARNING AND LEARNING STRATEGIES 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Anthony A. Wright 《Psychological science》1997,8(2):119-123
Abstract —Concept teaming and learning strategies of pigeons were manipulated in a matching-Co-sample task. Groups of 4 pigeons responded either 0, J, 10, or 20 times to a sample stimulus, and then chose between a matching comparison stimulus and a nonmatching comparison stimulus. Tests with unfamiliar arrangements of the three training stimuli showed that learning was not by if-then rules. Tests with novel stimuli showed that as the number of sample responses increased, learning about the configural pattern of each display gave way to more teaming about the sample-comparison relationship and more concept learning. Pigeons making the most sample responses showed complete concept teaming. 相似文献
102.
In two previous studies on general and violent recidivism (Walters & Heilbrun, 2010; Walters, Knight, Grann, & Dahle, 2008), the summed composite antisocial facet of the Psychopathy Checklist displayed incremental validity relative to the other 3 facets (interpersonal, affective, lifestyle), whereas the other 3 facets generally failed to demonstrate incremental validity relative to the antisocial facet. Because summed composite scores do not account for ordinal item distributions, the 6 Walters et al. (2008) samples were reanalyzed with factor score composites derived from a 4-factor confirmatory factor analysis. The results, however, showed little change from what had been obtained earlier with summed composite scores. Two additional samples not previously included in any incremental validity analyses of the Psychopathy Checklist evidenced a 3-factor structure, with the lifestyle and antisocial facets merged into a single factor. This single factor displayed incremental validity relative to the interpersonal and affective facets, but the reverse was not true regardless of whether summed composite scores or factor score composites were used. A comparison of zero-order correlations from all 8 samples revealed that the antisocial summed composite score predicted significantly better than the summed composite scores for the other 3 facets and that a superordinate factor failed to improve on the performance of either the antisocial summed composite score or the antisocial factor score composite. 相似文献
103.
A volunteer program for undergraduate psychology students at a chronic mental health facility was evaluated. All 53 volunteers found the program beneficial, 96.2% reported improved understanding of psychopathology, 98.1% noted increased knowledge of the mental health care system, 86.8% reported increased interest in clinical psychology, and 47.2% felt less anxious about working with persons with mental illness as a result of the program. 相似文献
104.
105.
106.
In previous work we have defended the deprivation account of death’s badness against worries stemming from the Lucretian point that prenatal and posthumous nonexistence are deprivations of the same sort. In a recent article in this journal, Fred Feldman has offered an insightful critique of our Parfitian strategy for defending the deprivation account of death’s badness. Here we adjust, clarify, and defend our strategy for reply to Lucretian worries on behalf of the deprivation account. 相似文献
107.
There is evidence to suggest that humour is an important part of mate choice and that humour may serve as an indicator of genetic quality. The current study investigated how rated funniness from a video clip was related to an individual’s attractiveness as a short-term or long-term partner. We additionally tested for the presence of an attractiveness halo effect on humour ratings by comparing ratings of funniness from video clips, audio-only presentations, and photographs. We found that funniness was most strongly correlated with attractiveness for short-term relationships, especially in videos of males. We also found that attractiveness was related to funniness ratings differently across video, audio-only clips, and photographs. Relative to their rated funniness in the audio-only condition, with no appearance cues, attractive individuals were rated as funnier in video clips than less attractive individuals. An additional study demonstrated that ratings of flirtatiousness and funniness were strongly correlated. Perceived similarity between producing humour and flirting may explain why humour is more preferable in a short-term partner as flirting may be seen to signal proceptivity. The effects of attractiveness on humour judgement may also be explained by an association with flirtation as flirting may be most enjoyable when directed by attractive individuals. 相似文献
108.
Rebecca K. Frels Anthony J. Onwuegbuzie 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2013,91(2):184-194
The authors demonstrate how collecting quantitative data via psychometrically sound quantitative instruments during the qualitative interview process enhances interpretations by helping researchers better contextualize qualitative findings, specifically through qualitative dominant crossover mixed analyses. They provide an example of this strategy, whereby a baseline was established using a quantitative scale and normative data to help interpret qualitative interviews, resulting in what they call a mixed methods interview. Philosophical and practical implications are discussed. 相似文献
109.
The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship between psychological correlates and flow in tennis competition. Using a cross-sectional design, the sample of 261 junior tennis athletes showed moderate-to-strong correlations between flow (dispositional and state) and confidence (r disp = .59; r state = .35), imagery use (r disp = .58; r state = .32), and action control (r disp = .41; r state = .40), whereas absorption was orthogonal to flow. Commonality analysis revealed that imagery and confidence accounted for 34.2% of the variance in dispositional flow. The results are discussed for the development of intervention studies in order to enhance flow in competition. 相似文献
110.
Anthony J. Faber 《The American journal of family therapy》2013,41(1):73-84
This article discusses the importance of parental hierarchy in regard to meeting the developmental nurturing needs of the child. It builds on Stonefish's (2000) epigenetic model of hierarchical relationship development. Through complementary and supplementary relationships between the parent and child, the child is able to have his or her nurturing needs met. 相似文献