全文获取类型
收费全文 | 22015篇 |
免费 | 62篇 |
专业分类
22077篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 11篇 |
2022年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 22篇 |
2020年 | 25篇 |
2019年 | 32篇 |
2018年 | 3508篇 |
2017年 | 2847篇 |
2016年 | 2288篇 |
2015年 | 230篇 |
2014年 | 109篇 |
2013年 | 257篇 |
2012年 | 646篇 |
2011年 | 2446篇 |
2010年 | 2561篇 |
2009年 | 1519篇 |
2008年 | 1751篇 |
2007年 | 2241篇 |
2006年 | 78篇 |
2005年 | 268篇 |
2004年 | 221篇 |
2003年 | 165篇 |
2002年 | 127篇 |
2001年 | 55篇 |
2000年 | 75篇 |
1999年 | 40篇 |
1998年 | 51篇 |
1997年 | 42篇 |
1996年 | 32篇 |
1995年 | 24篇 |
1994年 | 25篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 20篇 |
1991年 | 18篇 |
1990年 | 22篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 13篇 |
1986年 | 15篇 |
1985年 | 20篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 14篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 15篇 |
1977年 | 13篇 |
1976年 | 13篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 16篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
Anthony D. Pellegrini 《Journal of applied developmental psychology》1983,4(4):389-397
Two studies were conducted to determined the effects of specific learning centers on preschoolers' functional uses of language. In Study 1, preschoolers in three separate same-age classrooms (2-, 3-, and 5-year-olds) were observed on a time sampling scheduling during their free play periods for four weeks. Preschoolers' individual utterances were coded as serving one or more than one function; functions were defined following Halliday. The housekeeping and block centers tended to elicit the most individual and multifunctional utterances. The fantasy play and social interaction among children in these centers were thought to be responsible for this mature use of language.In Study 2, same-age and same-sex dyads from two age groups (4- and 5-year-olds) were observed in experimental settings playing with blocks alone and with dramatic props alone. Both contexts elicited a predominance of imaginative language, though the dramatic context elicited more imaginative language than the blocks. With age, children generated more individual functions and multifunctional utterances. 相似文献
52.
53.
Two strategies used to test prerequisite relationships among intellectual skills were employed to examine the sequence of three types of subtraction tasks. A psychometric validation strategy yielded results similar to an instructional validation strategy. This finding provides support for the use of the more efficient psychometric approach for validating large numbers of learning hierarchies in relatively short periods of time. 相似文献
54.
55.
Fazio, Zanna, and Cooper (Dissonance and self-perception: An integrative view on each theory's proper domain of application. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1977, 13, 464–479) specified mutually exclusive domains of application for dissonance and self-perception theory and used a misattribution-of-arousal procedure to distinguish between dissonance reduction and self-perception processes. Because their proposed limitations of the domains of the two theories and their use of the misattribution procedure are not directly derivable from earlier statements of either theory, it may be best to regard their analysis as a new theory, rather than as a conciliation of the parent theories. New analyses based on the Fazio et al. results indicate that their data provide an insufficient basis for preferring their theory to earlier versions of dissonance and self-perception theories. Dissonance and self-perception: An integrative view of each theory's proper domain of application. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1977, 13, 464–479. 相似文献
56.
57.
58.
Michael W. Trace Anthony J. Cuvo Judith L. Criswell 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1977,10(1):85-92
A program was designed to teach coin equivalence to mentally retarded adolescents. Coin equivalence was defined as choosing several different combinations of coins to equal specified target values. A pretest-posttest matched-groups design was employed with an experimental group receiving the monetary training, and a no-training control group. A multiple baseline across coin-counting responses was also incorporated in the experimental group. Training was divided into six stages, each teaching one specific method of combining coins to equal 10 target values from 5¢ through 50¢. A three-component response chain was used, requiring (a) naming, (b) selecting and counting, and (c) depositing target monetary values into a coin machine. Experimental subjects improved significantly in coin equivalence performance and maintained their skill on follow up tests; control subjects did not. 相似文献
59.
The purpose of the present study was to test the validity of three hypotheses previously offered to explain the attenuating effects of pacifistic noncooperation on instigative aggression. During part one of the experiment all subjects served as instigators; in the second part they actually set shocks for the target. Subjects instigated either a cooperative or noncooperative confederate, and either did or did not have veto power over the confederate's shock selections. Results of the study indicated that all hypotheses, norm-conformity, compliance, and maintenance of power, were viable for subgroups of subjects. Results were discussed in terms of individual differences and the temporarily inhibiting effects of passive resistance. 相似文献
60.
It was hypothesized that if a behavior is freely engaged in with the expectancy that an aversive consequence will follow, the intrinsic motivation to perform that behavior will increase when the aversive consequence is no longer present. Subjects either worked on a list of anagrams while receiving electric shock on a VR schedule, while being threatened by electric shock, or in the absence of shock. The experimenter then left the subject alone for 10 minutes with a new list of anagrams. Subjects in the shock condition solved more anagrams on the posttest than subjects in the threat-of-shock and noshock conditions. Subjects in both the shock condition and the threat-of-shock condition reported that they spent more of their free time on the anagrams than subjects in the no-shock condition. 相似文献