首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   171篇
  免费   3篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   6篇
  2019年   3篇
  2018年   5篇
  2017年   3篇
  2016年   4篇
  2015年   2篇
  2014年   2篇
  2013年   18篇
  2012年   3篇
  2011年   6篇
  2010年   5篇
  2009年   4篇
  2008年   9篇
  2007年   8篇
  2006年   10篇
  2005年   4篇
  2004年   3篇
  2003年   4篇
  2002年   8篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   7篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   6篇
  1993年   3篇
  1991年   5篇
  1990年   2篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   1篇
  1981年   4篇
  1979年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
  1974年   1篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   1篇
  1971年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1966年   1篇
  1957年   1篇
  1955年   1篇
  1949年   1篇
  1937年   2篇
  1934年   1篇
  1932年   1篇
  1931年   1篇
  1924年   1篇
  1921年   1篇
  1919年   1篇
排序方式: 共有174条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
Complex-span (working-memory-capacity) tasks are among the most successful predictors of intelligence. One important contributor to this relationship is the ability to efficiently employ cues for the retrieval from secondary memory. Presumably, intelligent individuals can considerably restrict their memory search sets by using such cues and can thereby improve recall performance. We here test this assumption by experimentally manipulating the validity of retrieval cues. When memoranda are drawn from the same semantic category on two successive trials of a verbal complex-span task, the category is a very strong retrieval cue on its first occurrence (strong-cue trial) but loses some of its validity on its second occurrence (weak-cue trial). If intelligent individuals make better use of semantic categories as retrieval cues, their recall accuracy suffers more from this loss of cue validity. Accordingly, our results show that less variance in intelligence is explained by recall accuracy on weak-cue compared with strong-cue trials.  相似文献   
53.
We examined how a highly familiar environmental space--one's city of residence--is represented in memory. Twenty-six participants faced a photo-realistic virtual model of their hometown and completed a task in which they pointed to familiar target locations from various orientations. Each participant's performance was most accurate when he or she was facing north, and errors increased as participants' deviation from a north-facing orientation increased. Pointing errors and latencies were not related to the distance between participants' initial locations and the target locations. Our results are inconsistent with accounts of orientation-free memory and with theories assuming that the storage of spatial knowledge depends on local reference frames. Although participants recognized familiar local views in their initial locations, their strategy for pointing relied on a single, north-oriented reference frame that was likely acquired from maps rather than experience from daily exploration. Even though participants had spent significantly more time navigating the city than looking at maps, their pointing behavior seemed to rely on a north-oriented mental map.  相似文献   
54.
Relevant data as to the sex of the first figure drawn in the DAP were pooled from 19 studies using 5- to 18-year old Ss. 83% of the 4989 boys and 78% of the 4443 girls showed self-sex preference. However, considering age in detail the self-sex proportion of boys significantly exceeds that of girls only from the 11th year on. A significant difference of opposite direction was found with the 7-year olds, but it is impossible so far to determine whether this is an artifact. Results are tentatively explained in terms of the culturally determined learning of sex-role identification.  相似文献   
55.
56.
A new combined temporal logic called synchronized linear-time temporal logic (SLTL) is introduced as a Gentzen-type sequent calculus. SLTL can represent the n-Cartesian product of the set of natural numbers. The cut-elimination and completeness theorems for SLTL are proved. Moreover, a display sequent calculus ??SLTL is defined.  相似文献   
57.
The trilattice SIXTEEN3\textit{SIXTEEN}_3 is a natural generalization of the well-known bilattice FOUR2\textit{FOUR}_2. Cut-free, sound and complete sequent calculi for truth entailment and falsity entailment in SIXTEEN3\textit{SIXTEEN}_3 are presented.  相似文献   
58.
This article seeks to address the following two questions: a) What are the sources of authority within your church, and how is authority exercised? b) In your church's understanding, what are the bases for maintaining union and what are the limits of sustainable diversity?  相似文献   
59.
Heinrich Wansing 《Synthese》2006,150(3):459-482
Model-theoretic proofs of functional completenes along the lines of [McCullough 1971, Journal of Symbolic Logic 36, 15–20] are given for various constructive modal propositional logics with strong negation.  相似文献   
60.
罗姆巴赫的结构现象学思想与东方哲学有着绝妙的契合。在其核心著作《结构存在论》一开篇,罗姆巴赫就明确地通过对老子哲学的阐释来说明他的“结构”概念。无论在艺术,伦理学和宗教信仰还是在科学活动中,结构的构造状态都被视为普全的基本形式;而这种向分支领域扩展的基础在于对现象学视域内存在论的结构分析。同时,他也强调结构只能在活的历史关联中被描述,不存在最终有效的静态结构范畴。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号