全文获取类型
收费全文 | 235篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
237篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 15篇 |
2016年 | 10篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 7篇 |
2013年 | 37篇 |
2012年 | 10篇 |
2011年 | 14篇 |
2010年 | 7篇 |
2009年 | 6篇 |
2008年 | 12篇 |
2007年 | 14篇 |
2006年 | 8篇 |
2005年 | 6篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 10篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1926年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有237条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
161.
Sibling relationships are argued to offer a rich social context for the development of young children’s theory of mind (ToM). There is evidence that the presence of siblings, particularly older siblings, may promote preschoolers’ ToM, but it has not been investigated among toddlers. This study evaluated differences in early manifestations of ToM between toddlers in different sibling compositions: 1) without a sibling, 2) with younger siblings only, and 3) with older siblings only. The sample included 227 toddlers who completed 2 tasks assessing their understanding of discrepant desires and visual perspectives. Results indicated that only children and those who had older siblings did not differ from each other on either task. However, on both tasks, toddlers with younger siblings performed significantly more poorly compared with those with older siblings. Moreover, children with younger siblings demonstrated poorer visual-perspective understanding compared with only children. It may be critical to take siblings’ birth order into account when investigating their respective effects on each other’s ToM, especially in toddlerhood. 相似文献
162.
There is growing evidence that, faced with a complex environment, participants subdivide the incoming information into small perceptual units, called chunks. Although statistical properties have been identified as playing a key role in chunking, we wanted to determine whether perceptual (repetitions) and positional (initial units) features might provide immediate guidance for the parsing of information into chunks. Children aged 5 and 8 years were exposed to sequences of 3, 4, or 5 colours. Sequence learning was assessed either through an explicit generation test (Experiment 1) or through a recognition test (Experiment 2). Experiment 1 showed that perceptual and positional saliencies benefited learning and that sensitivity to repetitions was age dependent and permitted the formation of longer chunks (trigrams) in the oldest children. Experiment 2 suggested that children became sensitive to perceptual and positional saliencies regardless of age and that the both types of saliencies supported the formation of longer chunks in the oldest children. The discussion focuses on the multiple factors intervening in sequence learning and their differential effects as a function of the instructions used at test to assess sequence learning. 相似文献
163.
Neha A. John-Henderson Sarah E. Williams Ryan C. Brindle Annie T. Ginty 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2018,109(4):694-707
Stress-related sleep disturbances are common, and poor sleep quality can negatively affect health. Previous work indicates that early-life adversity is associated with compromised sleep quality later in life, but it is unknown whether it predicts greater declines in sleep quality during stressful life transitions. We propose and test a conceptual model whereby individuals who reported experiencing greater levels of child maltreatment would experience greater psychological distress during a stressful life transition, which in turn would contribute to greater declines in sleep quality, relative to their quality of sleep before the stressful transition. Controlling for potential confounding variables (e.g., age, gender), structural equation modelling demonstrated that psychological distress experienced during a stressful transition (i.e., beginning life at university) mediated the relationship between childhood emotional neglect and changes in sleep quality. The hypothesized model demonstrated a good overall fit to the data, χ 2 (15) = 17.69, p = .279, CFI = .99, TLI = .97, SRMR = .04, RMSEA = .04 (90% CI <0.001–0.09). Emotional neglect (β = .22) was positively associated with psychological distress which in turn was positively associated with poor sleep quality (β = .31) during a stressful transition. Future research should aim to understand the specific stressors in the university environment that are most challenging to individuals who faced early-life emotional maltreatment. These findings will help inform interventions to facilitate adaptation to a new environment and improve sleep quality for these university students. 相似文献
164.
Tiffany A. Stauch Joshua B. Plavnick Sudha Sankar Annie C. Gallagher 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2018,51(3):647-666
Few interventions focus on teaching social skills to adolescents with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and intellectual disabilities (ID) that are consistently used during interactions with peers ( Carter et al., 2014). The present study evaluated the effects of video‐based group instruction (VGI) on the acquisition of social perception skills of five adolescents with ASD or ID in a public school setting. Social perception involves observing affective behaviors of others, discriminating relevant environmental stimuli, and differentially reinforcing the affective behavior of another person. Typically developing peers supported VGI implementation as social partners for participants. A multiple probe design across behaviors demonstrated the effectiveness of VGI for teaching social perception skills. Four of five participants acquired and maintained the targeted social perception skills, and we observed some transfer to a nontreatment setting. Results of this study suggest VGI may support the acquisition of social perception among adolescents with ASD or ID. 相似文献
165.
The objective of the present study was to show that the use of adversative and conclusive connectives to mark off the prototypical schema of argumentative text begins to set in at approximately the age of 10 or 11. Based on Adam's (1992) proposals, we constituted an argumentative text with two blocks of arguments separated by an adversative instruction (the connective but or an equivalent) and followed by a conclusion introduced by a conclusive instruction (the connective thus or an equivalent). Four revising tasks (insertion or substitution with or without five connectives) have been used to asses children's knowledge of the argumentative schema and the use of connectives that punctuate them. The study of good and erroneous locations (concerning the placement of but and thus) showed that there was some regularity in the choices made by the children. The main result of this study shows that argumentative connectives are used differently by children aged 9 and children aged 10 or 11. The argumentative schema is used more consciously by 11-year-olds to guide revising tasks than by 10-year-olds. 相似文献
166.
Young children of color—especially boys—are at disproportionate risk for suspension and expulsion from early care and education settings. Infant and Early Childhood Mental Health Consultation (IECMHC) is an approach associated with lower than expected expulsion rates, but the mechanisms through which it may influence preschool expulsion are unknown. This paper reflects on the parallels between IECMHC and interventions created to reduce implicit bias. Based on interviews with leaders in IECMHC practice, implementation, and evaluation, a theoretical framework was created to articulate how IECMHC is hypothesized to affect expulsion by first reducing the influence of implicit bias on disciplinary decisions. Implications for practice and research are provided. 相似文献
167.
Marlyse F. Haward Annie Janvier Gregory P. Moore Naomi Laventhal Jessica T. Fry 《The American journal of bioethics : AJOB》2020,20(7):37-43
Abstract In a crisis, societal needs take precedence over a patient’s best interests. Triage guidelines, however, differ on whether limited resources should focus on maximizing lives or life-years. Choosing between these two approaches has implications for neonatology. Neonatal units have ventilators, some adaptable for adults. This raises the question of whether, in crisis conditions, guidelines for treating extremely premature babies should be altered to free-up ventilators. Some adults who need ventilators will have a survival rate higher than some extremely premature babies. But surviving babies will likely live longer, maximizing life-years. Empiric evidence demonstrates that these babies can derive significant survival benefits from ventilation when compared to adults. When “triaging” or choosing between patients, justice demands fair guidelines. Premature babies do not deserve special consideration; they deserve equal consideration. Solidarity is crucial but must consider needs specific to patient populations and avoid biases against people with disabilities and extremely premature babies. 相似文献
168.
The language of the unconscious: poetry and psychoanalysis 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Reiner A 《Psychoanalytic review》2008,95(4):597-624
169.
Annie Sweetnam 《The International journal of psycho-analysis》2007,88(6):1491-1506
Being surprised by beauty, being drawn to and moved by it and surrendering to its sensory nature are all part of beauty's emotionally transformative power. And yet, psychoanalysis has had little to say about such an enriching dimension of psychological life. The author highlights how experiencing beauty is a capacity that is not isolated but is part of the lifelong experience of being changed by and changing the experience of the world we live in. She suggests that the capacity to experience beauty takes different forms at distinct points in development and in psychoanalytic treatments. The author describes two clinical occasions from a case where an analytic pair struggled to develop a greater capacity to experience beauty over time. She discusses how a theory of the aesthetic conflict was useful in helping to understand her patient's inner object world but was not sufficient when it came to understanding the experience of beauty. She shows how for both patient and analyst, experiencing beauty's full effect required responding to the call toward emotional life, surrendering to the surprise and the unknown, and letting go of the mind's struggle to find meaning and opening up to transforming and being transformed through sensory experience. 相似文献
170.
Kaimal G Steinberg AG Ennis S Harasink SM Ewing R Li Y 《Journal of genetic counseling》2007,16(6):775-787
Few studies examine whether and how parental attitudes towards genetic testing change over time. In this study we interviewed
parents of 14 children with newly identified hearing loss at two time points: after referral to genetics and 1 year later.
Qualitative analyses of parental narratives indicate that parental attitudes did not change significantly over this time.
Parents who perceived genetic testing to be useful continued to value it after testing, while parents who did not perceive
it as being useful for their child’s future held the same view a year later. The only parents who changed their views regarding
the usefulness of genetic testing for hearing loss were those who reported that their children underwent significant changes
in their hearing loss or were faced with other life threatening conditions. Parents were also often unaware of the role of
the genetic counselor and how genetic counseling could help address many of their lingering questions and concerns. These
emergent themes indicate the need for geneticists and genetic counselors to be aware of and sensitized to the questions and
attitudes that bring parents to a genetic evaluation, as well as the reasons why parents may not follow up with genetic testing
for hearing loss when recommended. 相似文献