全文获取类型
收费全文 | 371篇 |
免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
372篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 15篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 19篇 |
2013年 | 33篇 |
2012年 | 14篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 16篇 |
2009年 | 10篇 |
2008年 | 15篇 |
2007年 | 13篇 |
2006年 | 18篇 |
2005年 | 8篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 18篇 |
2002年 | 15篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有372条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
191.
Susan C. Somerville Annette Capuani-Shumaker 《The British journal of developmental psychology》1984,2(4):315-328
Thirty-two 3 and 32 4-year-olds were tested on two tasks which required them to infer the location of a concealed object. In the hiding tusk the child watched while an object was hidden and then attempted to find it; in the finding tusk the child watched while one object was found and then attempted to find a duplicate of it in the same place. Each sequence of events that the child watched ruled out two of the four locations in a linear array as possible hiding places for the object, but did not specify which of the remaining two contained it. The children did use the information conveyed by the sequences of events to decide where to search. Older children generally did better than younger children both in their first choices and in their second choices when the first, although correct, did not locate the object. However, neither age group made the correct choice of a second location consistently. This suggested that the children encountered mnemonic or logical difficulties, or both, because of the fact that two locations were equally compatible with a given sequence of events. 相似文献
192.
193.
194.
195.
Annette Streeck-Fischer 《American journal of psychoanalysis》2015,75(4):438-453
Adolescence is a period of instability caused by biological changes and restructuring of the personality. An immigration background renders the process of identity formation even more difficult or fragile, with an additional burden coming from persecution and harassment. Three case studies of mentally disturbed adolescents with different immigration backgrounds illustrate the problems in diagnosis and psychotherapy. All three cases share a common feature—the particular influence of the native country on the psychic disorder of the adolescent, be it a suitable target of narcissistic self-aggrandizement, a reactivated metaphor of the past or a deposited conflict. I point out and discuss the danger of diagnostic colonization and activation of perpetrator-victim constellations—such as the Nazi past in the present. Offering a transcultural transitional space as a container yields a therapeutic approach to the different worlds of these borderland adolescents. 相似文献
196.
197.
Annette M. B. de Groot 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1987,39(4):721-756
Four experiments are reported in which the magnitude of the associative-priming effect is manipulated by varying both the experimental task and the conditions within a given task. The between-task manipulation involved a comparison of priming effects in length categorization on the one hand and lexical decision on the other. The within-task manipulation involved varying the type of letter strings presented as the nonword stimuli in lexical decisions. Pseudowords and two types of orthographically illegal letter strings—namely, consonant strings and random letter strings—served as nonwords. Finally, the effect of varying the proportion of pseudowords to illegal nonwords across four sets of experimental materials was investigated. On the whole, these within-and between-task manipulations turned out to be very effective ways of affecting the size of the associative-priming effect. Also, the size of the lexicality effect (the difference between the RTs to word and nonword stimuli) varied systematically with these within-and between-task manipulations. The results are discussed in relation to both Shulman and Davison's (1977) parallel-processing model and Balota and Chumbley's (1984) account of lexical decision. 相似文献
198.
Harald Lachnit Annette Kinder 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology》2000,53(3):209-224
Three Pavlovian conditioning experiments with human participants are reported, which investigated whether common or separate stimulus representations are involved in solving nonlinear discrimination tasks in different response systems. In our experiments we made use of a negative transfer effect between positive and negative patterning. Experiment 1 specified the conditions under which such a negative transfer effect occurs in human eyelid conditioning. Experiments 2 and 3 investigated whether a similar effect also occurs if two response systems- the eyelid and the skin conductance response system- are trained with trials of both types being randomly interleaved. The presence or absence of a negative transfer effect indicates whether or not the stimulus representations involved in the two conditioning processes overlap. The findings are discussed within the framework of a neuropsychological model of hippocampal function. The results suggest that the representations are distinct and thus support the idea of acquired equivalence and distinctiveness of stimulus representations. 相似文献
199.
200.
Gillian B. Yeo Tamma Sorbello Annette Koy Luke D. Smillie 《Motivation and emotion》2008,32(4):296-309
Goal orientation theories were used to generate predictions regarding the moderating effect of goal orientation profiles on
task performance growth trajectories. Participants were given multiple trials of practice on an air traffic control task.
Analyses were conducted using growth curve modeling. As expected, individuals with high performance-approach orientation improved
their task performance scores faster than their counterparts. The interaction between mastery and performance-avoid orientations
moderated the performance growth curve such that individuals with high mastery and low performance-avoid orientation improved
their performance at the fastest rate. The interaction between performance-approach and performance-avoid orientations also
moderated the performance growth curve. Individuals with low performance-approach and high performance-avoid orientation improved
their performance at the slowest rate. These findings contribute to theory and practice by elucidating how various combinations
of goal orientations influence the rate of skill acquisition.
相似文献
Gillian B. YeoEmail: |