全文获取类型
收费全文 | 119篇 |
免费 | 0篇 |
专业分类
119篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 1篇 |
2019年 | 2篇 |
2018年 | 2篇 |
2017年 | 3篇 |
2016年 | 3篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 3篇 |
2013年 | 13篇 |
2012年 | 1篇 |
2011年 | 8篇 |
2010年 | 3篇 |
2009年 | 2篇 |
2008年 | 3篇 |
2007年 | 3篇 |
2006年 | 4篇 |
2005年 | 3篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 8篇 |
2002年 | 1篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 3篇 |
1964年 | 3篇 |
1959年 | 1篇 |
1958年 | 1篇 |
1950年 | 1篇 |
1937年 | 1篇 |
1935年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有119条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
Nicole Roberts Zofia Czajkowska George Radiotis Annett Körner 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2013,20(2):209-214
Non-melanoma skin cancer (NMSC) is the most common cancer world-wide, yet few studies have investigated the psychosocial implications of this disease. The current study provides an account of the distress experienced by patients with NMSC and the coping strategies they use to deal with the NMSC diagnosis. The results demonstrate that 19 % of the patients experience significant levels of psychological distress. Furthermore, those who engaged in avoidant coping strategies (specifically behavioural avoidance) were more likely to experience distress. These findings highlight the importance of further investigating this patient population, since patients report difficulties adjusting to this disease and its treatment. 相似文献
72.
Background
Despite changes in the regulations concerning outpatient psychotherapy in Germany, relatively few patients with substance use disorders receive this type of treatment.Objectives
The results of a recent study among stimulant users are taken as an example to illustrate the psychotherapeutic needs of patients with substance use disorders.Materials and methods
A total of 392 individuals with active use of amphetamine and/or methamphetamine were included using a broad range of recruitment strategies. In a mixed methods approach, qualitative as well as quantitative data were gathered regarding the history of substance use, the motives for use, and traumatic experiences during childhood.Results
Psychological distress was a frequent motive even at the initiation of stimulant use. About two thirds of the participants (65.1?%) reported at least one form of childhood trauma (sexual, physical or emotional abuse, emotional or physical neglect).Conclusion
The findings suggest a high need for psychotherapy among patients with substance use disorders. It seems important to further improve the information as well as the training of psychotherapists to work with these clients. 相似文献73.
74.
C W Kienhorst E J de Wilde J van den Bout M I Broese van Groenou R F Diekstra W H Wolters 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》1990,20(2):101-112
From nearly 10,000 secondary education students in The Netherlands, aged 14-20, data were collected on the prevalence of suicide attempts and recent thoughts about suicide and death. In this sample, 3.3% of the girls and 1.3% of the boys indicated that they had attempted suicide. The youngest students already reported as many attempts as the eldest, which is remarkable. Differences between boys and girls and between various types of schools were not substantial. We also found that 5.2% of the girls and 2.2% of the boys currently had thoughts of suicide. Again, there was no substantial difference as regards sex, type of school, and age. There was, however, a (small) difference between boys and girls concerning thoughts of death--namely, 9.3% of the girls versus 4.8% of the boys. 相似文献
75.
76.
77.
John Annett 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1966,18(3):273-274
The aim of this paper is to point out some implications of implicit or explicit payoffs in reaction time experiments. In any psychophysical task the subject is given a set of instructions which define the desired performance, as it were the rules of the game. Instructions may be supplemented by knowledge of results which further define the required performance. When performance admits of being “good” or “bad,” however wide these limits, the instructions may be said to define, explicitly or implicitly, a system of payoffs. In reaction time experiments the subject is instructed, amongst other things, to respond as soon as possible after the stimulus but never before it. These instructions could equally well be represented by a system of payoffs and Figure 1 illustrates the kind of payoff system normally implied in these instructions. If S represents the time at which the physical stimulus occurs the graph shows a fixed penalty for anticipations (response before S), maximum positive score for responses immediately following S declining at an arbitrary rate as a function of elapsed time between S and the response until an unacceptably long time involves a constant maximum penalty. Some experimenters choose to treat very short RT's as anticipations. This is represented by the dashed line in Figure 1, but it should be clear that payoff values are, to a certain extent, arbitrary. 相似文献
78.
79.
80.
Annett Schirmer Christy Reece Claris Zhao Erik Ng Esther Wu Shih‐Cheng Yen 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2015,106(1):107-132
Casual social touch influences emotional perceptions, attitudes, and behaviours of interaction partners. We asked whether these influences extend to third‐party observers. To this end, we developed the Social Touch Picture Set comprising line drawings of dyadic interactions, half of which entailed publicly acceptable casual touch and half of which served as no‐touch controls. In Experiment 1, participants provided basic image norms by rating how frequently they observed a displayed touch gesture in everyday life and how comfortable they were observing it. Results implied that some touch gestures were observed more frequently and with greater comfort than others (e.g., handshake vs. hug). All gestures, however, obtained rating scores suitable for inclusion in Experiments 2 and 3. In Experiment 2, participants rated perceived valence, arousal, and likeability of randomly presented touch and no‐touch images without being explicitly informed about touch. Image characters seemed more positive, aroused, and likeable when they touched as compared to when they did not touch. Image characters seemed more negative and aroused, but were equally likeable, when they received touch as compared to when there was no physical contact. In Experiment 3, participants passively viewed touch and no‐touch images while their eye movements were recorded. Differential gazing at touch as compared to no‐touch images emerged within the first 500 ms following image exposure and was largely restricted to the characters' upper body. Gazing at the touching body parts (e.g., hands) was minimal and largely unaffected by touch, suggesting that touch processing occurred outside the focus of visual attention. Together, these findings establish touch as an important visual cue and provide novel insights into how this cue modulates socio‐emotional processing in third‐party observers. 相似文献