全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2605篇 |
免费 | 31篇 |
专业分类
2636篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 25篇 |
2023年 | 47篇 |
2022年 | 37篇 |
2021年 | 59篇 |
2020年 | 88篇 |
2019年 | 103篇 |
2018年 | 117篇 |
2017年 | 118篇 |
2016年 | 112篇 |
2015年 | 89篇 |
2014年 | 109篇 |
2013年 | 296篇 |
2012年 | 163篇 |
2011年 | 152篇 |
2010年 | 80篇 |
2009年 | 100篇 |
2008年 | 100篇 |
2007年 | 101篇 |
2006年 | 76篇 |
2005年 | 73篇 |
2004年 | 59篇 |
2003年 | 64篇 |
2002年 | 56篇 |
2001年 | 32篇 |
2000年 | 32篇 |
1999年 | 23篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 18篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 14篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 16篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 8篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1970年 | 7篇 |
1969年 | 7篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有2636条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
951.
Visual attention is considered nowadays a paramount ability both in Cognitive Sciences and in Cognitive Vision to bridge the
gap between perception and higher level reasoning functions, such as scene interpretation and decision making. Bottom-up gaze
shifting is the main mechanism used by humans when exploring a scene without a specific task. In this paper we investigated
which criteria allow for the generation of plausible fixation clusters by analysing experimental data of human subjects. We
suggest that fixations should be grouped in cliques whose saliency can be assessed through an innovation factor encompassing
bottom-up cues, proximity, direction and memory components. 相似文献
952.
953.
Sandra T. Sigmon Anna G. Cassel Rachel F. S. Dawson Janell G. Schartel Lindsay R. Owings Geoffrey L. Thorpe 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》2009,27(3):176-187
The impact of the changing seasons on mood and behavior (i.e., seasonality) has long been of interest to researchers. Recently,
researchers have begun to look beyond biological explanations to investigate psychological variables that may play a role
in the development of seasonal affective disorder (SAD) and subsyndromal (S-SAD) levels of seasonality. Rumination, in particular,
has received initial support as a process that predicts more severe levels of winter depression. In the first study, we assessed
the effects of rumination, current weather conditions, current level of depression, and attitudes toward weather conditions
as predictors of seasonality. In addition to weather conditions, weather attitudes, and current depression, rumination emerged
as a significant predictor of seasonality in females but not for males. In the second study, we followed individuals categorized
as being high (S-SAD) and low in seasonality over time and assessed for depression at Time 1 and Time 2. Rumination, in addition
to current weather conditions and weather attitudes, emerged as a significant predictor of depression for females in the S-SAD
group. The results of these studies provide support for the role of rumination in seasonal depression, particularly for females.
This type of research may contribute to attempts to better explain gender differences in seasonal depression. 相似文献
954.
Gerhard Blickle Alexander H. Witzki Paula B. Schneider 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》2009,74(2):181-189
Career success of early employees was analyzed from a power perspective and a developmental network perspective. In a predictive field study with 112 employees mentoring support and mentors’ power were assessed in the first wave, employees’ networking was assessed after two years, and career success (i.e. income and hierarchical position) and career satisfaction were assessed after three years. Networking was the most robust predictor of career success. Mentoring received predicted career satisfaction and its effects on objective career success were mediated by networking. Further, mentoring by a powerful mentor predicted objective career success beyond networking. Based on previous findings we argue that these findings underscore the critical relationship between early career employees’ networking behaviors and mentoring received. 相似文献
955.
We comment on Goldstein, Davidoff, and Roberson’s replication and extension (Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 219-238 [2009]) of our study of the effect of toddlers’ color term knowledge on their categorical perception (CP) of color (Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 114-141 [2005]). First, we discuss how best to assess color term knowledge when concerned with the effects of language on color CP. A reanalysis of our data indicates that even toddlers who do not know the terms for the relevant focal colors still show CP. Second, we comment on Goldstein and colleagues’ finding of blue-purple CP, as we did, but not of blue-green CP in Himba toddlers. We present contrasting data from Wright (unpublished PhD thesis, University of Surrey, 2006) that demonstrates blue-green CP in Himba toddlers. Finally, we discuss the limitations of the approach taken by all of these investigations and discuss theoretical accounts of the origin and nature of color CP. 相似文献
956.
Susan Schneider 《Synthese》2009,170(2):235-250
According to the language of thought (LOT) approach and the related computational theory of mind (CTM), thinking is the processing
of symbols in an inner mental language that is distinct from any public language. Herein, I explore a deep problem at the
heart of the LOT/CTM program—it has yet to provide a plausible conception of a mental symbol. 相似文献
957.
The well-studied link between psychotic traits and creativity is a subject of much debate. The present study investigated the extent to which schizotypic personality traits – as measured by O-LIFE (Oxford–Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences) – equip healthy individuals to engage as groups in everyday tasks. From a sample of 69 students, eight groups of four participants – comprised of high, medium, or low-schizotypy individuals – were assembled to work as a team to complete a creative problem-solving task. Predictably, high scorers on the O-LIFE formulated a greater number of strategies to solve the task, indicative of creative divergent thinking. However, for task success (as measured by time taken to complete the problem) an inverted U shaped pattern emerged, whereby high and low-schizotypy groups were consistently faster than medium schizotypy groups. Intriguing data emerged concerning leadership within the groups, and other tangential findings relating to anxiety, competition and motivation were explored. These findings challenge the traditional cliché that psychotic personality traits are linearly related to creative performance, and suggest that the nature of the problem determines which thinking styles are optimally equipped to solve it. 相似文献
958.
Stefan Stieger Anna V. PreyssMartin Voracek 《Personality and individual differences》2012,52(1):51-55
Research on romantic jealousy and self-esteem mostly relies on the measurement of explicit (i.e., conscious, deliberate) aspects, without taking recent developments of the measurement of implicit (i.e., automatic) aspects into account. In this study (N = 154), we applied several measures of romantic jealousy and self-esteem (explicit, implicit), finding sex-specific as well as measurement-specific effects. Men (but not women) higher in jealousy had lower explicit self-esteem, whereas women (but not men) higher in jealousy had higher implicit self-esteem, but only when using the Implicit Association Test (whereas not the Initial Preference Task) for measuring implicit self-esteem. Individuals with damaged (i.e., low explicit and high implicit) self-esteem were more jealous than those with fragile (i.e., high explicit and low implicit) self-esteem. This differential effect was due to higher implicit self-esteem among women, whereas lower explicit self-esteem among men. These novel findings not only add to the expanding literature on romantic jealousy research, but also to research on self-esteem discrepancies. 相似文献
959.
960.