全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1840篇 |
免费 | 133篇 |
专业分类
1973篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 41篇 |
2022年 | 32篇 |
2021年 | 55篇 |
2020年 | 75篇 |
2019年 | 90篇 |
2018年 | 111篇 |
2017年 | 98篇 |
2016年 | 98篇 |
2015年 | 77篇 |
2014年 | 96篇 |
2013年 | 248篇 |
2012年 | 145篇 |
2011年 | 121篇 |
2010年 | 59篇 |
2009年 | 73篇 |
2008年 | 83篇 |
2007年 | 78篇 |
2006年 | 54篇 |
2005年 | 54篇 |
2004年 | 46篇 |
2003年 | 48篇 |
2002年 | 37篇 |
2001年 | 15篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 14篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
1963年 | 2篇 |
1959年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有1973条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
53.
Anna M. Bardone-Cone Katherine A. Thompson Alexandra J. Miller 《Journal of personality》2020,88(1):59-75
In our review, we focus on self-related constructs in the context of eating disorders with four aims. First, we examine a variety of self-related constructs that have been theoretically and empirically linked to the development and course of eating disorders. In addition to the more well-researched constructs of self-esteem and self-efficacy, we also report on findings related to selflessness, contingent self-worth, self-objectification, ego-syntonicity, self-concept clarity, self-compassion, social comparison, self-oriented perfectionism/self-criticism, and narcissism. Second, we discuss self-related constructs that may be especially relevant to comorbidities common among those with eating disorders. Third, we review intervention and prevention programs where self-related constructs play a prominent role. Lastly, we share future research directions regarding self-related constructs and eating disorders that we believe will advance a deeper understanding of the role of the self in the eating disorders. 相似文献
54.
Anja C. Zeller Daniela Conrad Anna Schneider Alexander Behnke Anett Pfeiffer Gerrit F. Blum Sarah Wilker Thomas Elbert Iris-Tatjana Kolassa 《Aggressive behavior》2020,46(6):465-475
Individuals who perpetrate violence may likely perceive violence as appealing and infliction of violence to derive pleasure is termed as appetitive aggression. Individuals who were abducted as children into an armed group often experience a higher number of traumatic event types, that is traumatic load and are usually socialized in a violence-endorsing environment. This study aims to investigate the interaction between age at initial abduction with that of traumatic load, and their influence on appetitive aggression along with perpetration of violent acts by former members of an armed rebel group of both sexes. Semi-structured interviews were conducted among a target group of formerly abducted rebel-war survivors (including participants with and without combat experience) from Northern Uganda. Participants included 596 women and 570 men with N = 1,166 (Mage = 32.58, SDage = 9.76, range: 18–80 years). We conducted robust linear regression models to investigate the influence of age at initial abduction, traumatic load, combat experience, and biological sex on appetitive aggression as well as their perpetrated violent acts. Our study shows, appetitive aggression and the number of perpetrated violent acts were specifically increased in individuals who were abducted young, experienced several traumatic events in their lifetime, and with previous combat experience. For perpetrated violence men showed increased levels whereas for appetitive aggression the association was independent of biological sex. Therefore, early abducted individuals with a higher traumatic load, who have combat experience, need to be given special intervention to prevent any further violence. 相似文献
55.
Working memory training has been shown to improve performance on untrained working memory tasks in typically developing children, at least when compared to non‐adaptive training; however, there is little evidence that it improves academic outcomes. The lack of transfer to academic outcomes may be because children are only learning skills and strategies in a very narrow context, which they are unable to apply to other tasks. Metacognitive strategy interventions, which promote metacognitive awareness and teach children general strategies that can be used on a variety of tasks, may be a crucial missing link in this regard. In this double‐blind randomized controlled trial, 95 typically developing children aged 9–14 years were allocated to three cognitive training programmes that were conducted daily after‐school. One group received Cogmed working memory training, another group received concurrent Cogmed and metacognitive strategy training, and the control group received adaptive visual search training, which better controls for expectancy and motivation than non‐adaptive training. Children were assessed on four working memory tasks, reading comprehension, and mathematical reasoning before, immediately after, and 3 months after training. Working memory training improved working memory and mathematical reasoning relative to the control group. The improvements in working memory were maintained 3 months later, and these were significantly greater for the group that received metacognitive strategy training, compared to working memory training alone. Working memory training is a potentially effective educational intervention when provided in addition to school; however, future research will need to investigate ways to maintain academic improvements long term and to optimize metacognitive strategy training to promote far‐transfer. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://youtu.be/-7MML48ZFgw 相似文献
56.
Continental Philosophy Review - In the contemporary phenomenological literature it has been argued that it is possible to distinguish between two forms of selfhood: the “minimal” and... 相似文献
57.
58.
59.
Sex Roles - Different strategies of gender-fair language have been applied to reduce a male bias, which means the implicit belief that a word describing an undefined person describes a man. This... 相似文献
60.
Research on bilingualism and emotions has shown stronger emotional responses in the native language (L1) compared to a foreign language. We investigated the potential of purposeful second language (L2) use as a means of decreasing the experience of psychological distress. Native Swedish speakers read and answered questions about negative and neutral texts in their L1 (Swedish) and their L2 (English) and were asked to rate their level of distress before or after the questions. The texts and associated questions were either written in the same (within-language), or different languages (cross-language). We found that within-language trials when the text was written in participants’ native language (Swedish–Swedish) resulted in an increase of distress, whilst cross-language trials (Swedish–English) resulted in a decrease of distress. This implies that purposeful second language use can diminish levels of distress experienced following a negative event encoded in one's first language. 相似文献