全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3223篇 |
免费 | 50篇 |
专业分类
3273篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 22篇 |
2023年 | 26篇 |
2022年 | 16篇 |
2021年 | 40篇 |
2020年 | 75篇 |
2019年 | 106篇 |
2018年 | 101篇 |
2017年 | 99篇 |
2016年 | 138篇 |
2015年 | 79篇 |
2014年 | 90篇 |
2013年 | 406篇 |
2012年 | 158篇 |
2011年 | 171篇 |
2010年 | 101篇 |
2009年 | 91篇 |
2008年 | 99篇 |
2007年 | 138篇 |
2006年 | 114篇 |
2005年 | 99篇 |
2004年 | 104篇 |
2003年 | 94篇 |
2002年 | 84篇 |
2001年 | 39篇 |
2000年 | 55篇 |
1999年 | 48篇 |
1998年 | 36篇 |
1997年 | 40篇 |
1996年 | 40篇 |
1995年 | 46篇 |
1994年 | 39篇 |
1993年 | 28篇 |
1992年 | 31篇 |
1991年 | 25篇 |
1990年 | 26篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 19篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 21篇 |
1984年 | 25篇 |
1983年 | 29篇 |
1982年 | 34篇 |
1981年 | 19篇 |
1980年 | 25篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1978年 | 18篇 |
1977年 | 16篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1975年 | 17篇 |
1974年 | 16篇 |
排序方式: 共有3273条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
191.
Michael D. Mumford Ethan P. Waples Alison L. Antes Stephen T. Murphy Shane Connelly Ryan P. Brown 《Ethics & behavior》2013,23(5):351-378
An implicit goal of many interventions intended to enhance integrity is to minimize peoples' exposure to unethical events. The intent of the present effort was to examine if exposure to unethical practices in the course of one's work is related to ethical decision making. Accordingly, 248 doctoral students in the biological, health, and social sciences were asked to complete a field appropriate measure of ethical decision making. In addition, they were asked to complete measures examining the perceived acceptability of unethical events and a measure examining perceptions of ethical climate. When these criterion measures were correlated with a measure examining the frequency with which they had been exposed to unethical events in their day-to-day work, it was found that event exposure was strongly related to ethical decision making but less strongly related to climate perceptions and perceptions of event acceptability. However, these relationships were moderated by level of experience. The implications of these findings for practices intended to improve ethics are discussed. 相似文献
192.
Jillian O'Rourke Jeffrey Barnes Anna Deaton Kristopher Fulks Kristina Ryan David A. Rettinger 《Ethics & behavior》2013,23(1):47-64
What effect does witnessing other students cheat have on one's own cheating behavior? What roles do moral attitudes and neutralizing attitudes (justifications for behavior) play when deciding to cheat? The present research proposes a model of academic dishonesty which takes into account each of these variables. Findings from experimental (vignette) and survey methods determined that seeing others cheat increases cheating behavior by causing students to judge the behavior less morally reprehensible, not by making rationalization easier. Witnessing cheating also has unique effects, controlling for other variables. 相似文献
193.
This study investigated intimate partner violence (IPV) victims' emotion-focused coping efforts, as well as their retrospective ratings of the perceived helpfulness of these efforts, in the context of a longitudinal study of battered women's experiences over time. Four hundred and six primarily African American, low-income battered women who had experienced IPV within the previous 12 months were interviewed. Patterns of coping use and perceived helpfulness were explored. The correlation between prevalence of use of emotion-focused coping strategies and perceived helpfulness of these strategies was examined, and results showed that the strategies used by more battered women were less helpful in dealing with feelings about abuse. Implications for clinical interventions with battered women are discussed. 相似文献
194.
Jordan P. LaBouff Wade C. Rowatt Megan K. Johnson Michelle Thedford Jo‐Ann Tsang 《Journal for the scientific study of religion》2010,49(3):439-455
An implicit measure of religiousness‐spirituality (RS) was constructed and used in two studies. In Study 1, undergraduates completed a Religiousness‐Spirituality Implicit Association Test (RS‐IAT) and several self‐report measures of RS and related constructs (e.g., religious fundamentalism, authoritarianism). Informants rated the participants’ RS. The RS‐IAT was internally consistent. Implicit RS correlated positively with self‐reported RS, spiritual transcendence, spiritual experiences, religious fundamentalism, and intrinsic religiousness. Informant ratings correlated positively with participants’ self‐reported religiousness but not implicit RS. In Study 2, implicit RS accounted for unique variability in self‐reported attitudes toward gay men and lesbian women when controlling for self‐reported religiousness and right‐wing authoritarianism. These findings demonstrate that an implicit measure of trait RS explains some variability in attitudes that self‐report measures do not. An implicit measure of RS could advance the scientific study of religion beyond what is known from self‐report measures. 相似文献
195.
Scott R. Hinze Daniel G. Slaten William S. Horton Ryan Jenkins David N. Rapp 《Memory & cognition》2014,42(2):305-324
People rely on information they read even when it is inaccurate (Marsh, Meade, & Roediger, Journal of Memory and Language 49:519–536, 2003), but how ubiquitous is this phenomenon? In two experiments, we investigated whether this tendency to encode and rely on inaccuracies from text might be influenced by the plausibility of misinformation. In Experiment 1, we presented stories containing inaccurate plausible statements (e.g., “The Pilgrims’ ship was the Godspeed”), inaccurate implausible statements (e.g., . . . the Titanic), or accurate statements (e.g., . . . the Mayflower). On a subsequent test of general knowledge, participants relied significantly less on implausible than on plausible inaccuracies from the texts but continued to rely on accurate information. In Experiment 2, we replicated these results with the addition of a think-aloud procedure to elicit information about readers’ noticing and evaluative processes for plausible and implausible misinformation. Participants indicated more skepticism and less acceptance of implausible than of plausible inaccuracies. In contrast, they often failed to notice, completely ignored, and at times even explicitly accepted the misinformation provided by plausible lures. These results offer insight into the conditions under which reliance on inaccurate information occurs and suggest potential mechanisms that may underlie reported misinformation effects. 相似文献
196.
197.
Previous research indicates that peer victimization is tied to children's negative appearance evaluations. The current study examines whether early peer victimization is also prospectively related to objectified body consciousness. Six-hundred-and-two Swedish boys and girls answered questionnaires at age 10, and again at age 18. Main findings showed that being the target of peer victimization at age 10 was related to more habitual appearance monitoring and body shame at age 18. Gender moderated the relations between victimization and body shame, with victimized girls experiencing stronger body shame than victimized boys. Additionally, whereas boys experienced less body shame than girls, they were equally likely to monitor their appearance. In sum, this study provides preliminary support to the notion that peer victimization is involved in the processes by which young adolescents’ self-objectify. Future studies are warranted to further validate these findings. 相似文献
198.
Ryan Williams LaMothe 《Pastoral Psychology》2011,60(4):523-536
In this article, I offer a way of thinking about the interplay of political and religious subjectivities in persons’ conversion
experiences or faith journeys. More particularly, I contend that a person’s political and religious experiences interact such
that political experiences and ideas shape the person’s religious values and beliefs, and vice versa. Malcolm X’s story is
used to illustrate this interplay of subjectivities vis-à-vis the varied changes or conversions in his life. The major point
here is that the richness and depth of change in Malcolm X’s life (and other figures) are better grasped when we take into
account the dynamic intersection of the political and religious. 相似文献
199.
200.
Incarcerated mothers at a state prison participated in an eight-session parenting class designed to help them manage the stress
of separation from children and to improve communication patterns with children and home-caregivers. In comparison to a waitlist
control group (n = 46), inmates who received immediate intervention (n = 60) experienced less parenting distress regarding upcoming visitation experiences; however, waitlist and immediate treatment
groups did not differ on other intervention measures. Additional analyses contrasted pre- and post-intervention differences
on adjustment measures for inmates from either treatment condition who completed the parenting program (N = 90). After intervention, mothers reported reduced parenting stress, improved alliance with home caregivers, increased letter-writing,
and reduction of mental distress symptoms. Large drop-out rates in both subgroups may have reduced the benefits of the random
assignment used to form groups. Results support the value of interventions for incarcerated mothers that focus on ways to
manage the emotional distress and poor communication patterns associated with being a parent in prison. 相似文献